Confounding in epidemiology

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Confounding and effect modification
Advertisements

Case-control study 3: Bias and confounding and analysis Preben Aavitsland.
1 Matching EPIET introductory course Mahón, 2011.
Analytical epidemiology
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ)
Matching in Case-Control Designs EPID 712 Lecture 13 02/23/00 Megan O’Brien.
M2 Medical Epidemiology
Study Designs in Epidemiologic
EPID Introduction to Analysis and Interpretation of HIV/STD Data Confounding Manya Magnus, Ph.D. Summer 2001 adapted from M. O’Brien and P. Kissinger.
1 Confounding and Interaction: Part II  Methods to Reduce Confounding –during study design: »Randomization »Restriction »Matching –during study analysis:
Confounding and Interaction: Part II  Methods to reduce confounding –during study design: »Randomization »Restriction »Matching –during study analysis:
1 Case-Control Study Design Two groups are selected, one of people with the disease (cases), and the other of people with the same general characteristics.
Sensitivity Analysis for Observational Comparative Effectiveness Research Prepared for: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ)
Revisiting causal neighborhood effects on individual ischemic heart disease risk: a quasi-experimental analysis among Swedish siblings Juan Merlo In collaboration.
Chance, bias and confounding
Estimation and Reporting of Heterogeneity of Treatment Effects in Observational Comparative Effectiveness Research Prepared for: Agency for Healthcare.
Research Design: The Experimental Model and Its Variations
Confounding and Interaction: Part II
Sampling and Experimental Control Goals of clinical research is to make generalizations beyond the individual studied to others with similar conditions.
Categorical Data Analysis: Stratified Analyses, Matching, and Agreement Statistics Biostatistics March 2007 Carla Talarico.
Case-Control Studies. Feature of Case-control Studies 1. Directionality Outcome to exposure 2. Timing Retrospective for exposure, but case- ascertainment.
Cohort Studies Hanna E. Bloomfield, MD, MPH Professor of Medicine Associate Chief of Staff, Research Minneapolis VA Medical Center.
Chapter 12 Inferential Statistics Gay, Mills, and Airasian
Study Design and Analysis in Epidemiology: Where does modeling fit? Meaningful Modeling of Epidemiologic Data, 2010 AIMS, Muizenberg, South Africa Steve.
Case Control Study Manish Chaudhary BPH, MPH
Stratification and Adjustment
Cohort Study.
Unit 6: Standardization and Methods to Control Confounding.
The third factor Effect modification Confounding factor FETP India.
Advanced Statistics for Interventional Cardiologists.
Concepts of Interaction Matthew Fox Advanced Epi.
Epidemiologic Study Designs Nancy D. Barker, MS. Epidemiologic Study Design The plan of an empirical investigation to assess an E – D relationship. Exposure.
Epidemiology The Basics Only… Adapted with permission from a class presentation developed by Dr. Charles Lynch – University of Iowa, Iowa City.
Confounding 混杂偏倚 Michael Engelgau Shanghai FETP August 15, 2012.
Web of Causation; Exposure and Disease Outcomes Thomas Songer, PhD Basic Epidemiology South Asian Cardiovascular Research Methodology Workshop.
Study Designs Afshin Ostovar Bushehr University of Medical Sciences Bushehr, /4/20151.
ECON ECON Health Economic Policy Lab Kem P. Krueger, Pharm.D., Ph.D. Anne Alexander, M.S., Ph.D. University of Wyoming.
Lecture 6 Objective 16. Describe the elements of design of observational studies: (current) cohort studies (longitudinal studies). Discuss the advantages.
Confounding, Matching, and Related Analysis Issues Kevin Schwartzman MD Lecture 8a June 22, 2005.
Amsterdam Rehabilitation Research Center | Reade Multiple regression analysis Analysis of confounding and effectmodification Martin van de Esch, PhD.
COMH7202: EPIDEMIOLOGY III – INTERMEDIATE CONCEPTS Confounding & Effect Modification
Introduction to confounding and DAGs
Article Review Cara Carty 09-Mar-06. “Confounding by indication in non-experimental evaluation of vaccine effectiveness: the example of prevention of.
Estimating Causal Effects from Large Data Sets Using Propensity Scores Hal V. Barron, MD TICR 5/06.
Patricia Cohen, Ph.D. Henian Chen, M.D., Ph. D. Teaching Assistants Julie KranickSylvia Taylor Chelsea MorroniJudith Weissman Applied Epidemiologic Analysis.
Study Designs for Clinical and Epidemiological Research Carla J. Alvarado, MS, CIC University of Wisconsin-Madison (608)
Analytical epidemiology Disease frequency Study design: cohorts & case control Choice of a reference group Biases Alain Moren, 2006 Impact Causality Effect.
Educational Research Chapter 13 Inferential Statistics Gay, Mills, and Airasian 10 th Edition.
Case Control Study Dr. Ashry Gad Mohamed MB, ChB, MPH, Dr.P.H. Prof. Of Epidemiology.
Instructor Resource Chapter 14 Copyright © Scott B. Patten, Permission granted for classroom use with Epidemiology for Canadian Students: Principles,
11/20091 EPI 5240: Introduction to Epidemiology Confounding: concepts and general approaches November 9, 2009 Dr. N. Birkett, Department of Epidemiology.
Study designs. Kate O’Donnell General Practice & Primary Care.
Instructor Resource Chapter 15 Copyright © Scott B. Patten, Permission granted for classroom use with Epidemiology for Canadian Students: Principles,
Confounding and effect modification Epidemiology 511 W. A. Kukull November
Matching. Objectives Discuss methods of matching Discuss advantages and disadvantages of matching Discuss applications of matching Confounding residual.
Design of Clinical Research Studies ASAP Session by: Robert McCarter, ScD Dir. Biostatistics and Informatics, CNMC
POPLHLTH 304 Regression (modelling) in Epidemiology Simon Thornley (Slides adapted from Assoc. Prof. Roger Marshall)
Confounding Biost/Stat 579 David Yanez Department of Biostatistics University of Washington July 7, 2005.
Types of Studies. Aim of epidemiological studies To determine distribution of disease To examine determinants of a disease To judge whether a given exposure.
Educational Research Inferential Statistics Chapter th Chapter 12- 8th Gay and Airasian.
1 Study Design Imre Janszky Faculty of Medicine, ISM NTNU.
Purpose of Epi Studies Discover factors associated with diseases, physical conditions and behaviors Identify the causal factors Show the efficacy of intervening.
(www).
Methods of Presenting and Interpreting Information Class 9.
Validity in epidemiological research Deepti Gurdasani.
Lecture 3: Introduction to confounding (part 1)
Jeffrey E. Korte, PhD BMTRY 747: Foundations of Epidemiology II
Evaluating Effect Measure Modification
Confounders.
Effect Modifiers.
Presentation transcript:

Confounding in epidemiology Maura Pugliatti, MD, PhD Associate Professor of Neurology Dept. of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Unit of Clinical Neurology University of Sassari, Italy 1st International Course of Neuroepidemiology Chisinau, Moldova, 24-28 Sept. 2012

Definitions “Confounding, the situation in which an apparent effect of an exposure on risk is explained by its association with other factors, is probably the most important cause of spurious associations in observational epidemiology” BMJ Editorial: “The scandal of poor epidemiological research” BMJ 2004;329:868-869 “Bias of the estimated effect of an exposure on an outcome, due to the presence of a common cause of the exposure and the outcome” Porta, 2008

Overview Causality: central concern of epidemiology Confounding: central concern when establishing causality Four approaches to understand confounding Avoiding and controlling for confounding is essential in health research

Main application of epidemiology: Causality Main application of epidemiology: to identify etiologic (causal) associations between exposure(s) and outcome(s) ? Exposure Outcome

Adapted from: Maclure, M, Schneeweis S. Epidemiology 2001;12:114-122. Key biases in identifying causal effects: Causal Effect Random Error Confounding Information bias (misclassification) Selection bias Bias in inference Reporting & publication bias Bias in knowledge use RRcausal “truth” RRassociation Adapted from: Maclure, M, Schneeweis S. Epidemiology 2001;12:114-122.

Confounding: four approaches “Mixing of effects” Based on a priori criteria (classical approach) Data-based criteria “Counterfactual” and non-comparability approaches Overlapping

“Confounding is confusion, or mixing, of effects; the effect of the exposure is mixed together with the effect of another variable, leading to bias” Latin: “confundere” = “to mix together” Rothman KJ. Epidemiology. An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002

Association between birth order and Down Syndrome Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

Association between maternal age and Down Syndrome Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

Association between maternal age and Down Syndrome, stratified by birth order Data from Stark and Mantel (1966)

X C E C D E C D A factor is a confounder if 3 criteria are met: 1. A confounder must be causally or non-causally associated with the exposure in the source population (study base) being studied; E 2. A confounder must be a causal risk factor (or a surrogate measure of a cause) for the disease in the unexposed cohort; and C D 3. A confounder must not be an intermediate cause (not an intermediate step in the causal pathway between the exposure and the disease) X E C D

Confounder C E D C E D Exposure Disease (outcome) Intermediate cause Szklo M, Nieto JF. Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. Aspen Publishers, Inc., 2000. Gordis L. Epidemiology. Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 4th Edition.

Confounder: ‘parent’ of the exposure not ‘daughter’ of the exposure!!! Exposure Disease E D Confounder C

Confounding factor: Maternal Age C Birth Order Down Syndrome D E

Simple causal graphs E D C Maternal age (C) can confound the association between multivitamin use (E) and the risk of certain birth defects (D) Hernan MA, et al. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol 2002;155:176-84.

Complex causal graphs E D C U History of birth defects (C) may increase the chance of periconceptional vitamin intake (E). A genetic factor (U) could have been the cause of previous birth defects in the family, and could again cause birth defects in the current pregnancy (D) Hernan MA, et al. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol 2002;155:176-84.

Calcium supplementation More complicated causal graphs Physical Activity Smoking A B BMI C U E D Bone fractures Calcium supplementation Source: Hertz-Picciotto

A factor is a confounder if: a) the effect measure is homogeneous across the strata defined by the confounder and b) the crude and common stratum-specific (adjusted) effect measures are unequal (“lack of collapsibility”) Usually evaluated using 2x2 tables, and simple stratified analyses to compare crude effects with adjusted effects “Collapsibility is equality of stratum-specific measures of effect with the crude (collapsed), unstratified measure” Porta, 2008, Dictionary

Crude vs. Adjusted Effects Crude: does not take into account the effect of the confounder Adjusted: accounts for the confounder Mantel-Haenszel method estimator Multivariate analyses (e.g. logistic regression) Confounding is likely when: RRcrude =/= RRadjusted ORcrude =/= ORadjusted

Stratified Analysis Crude Crude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify by Confounder Calculate OR’s for each stratum If stratum-specific OR’s are similar, calculate adjusted OR (e.g. MH) ORCrude Stratum 1 Stratum 2 OR1 OR2 If Crude OR =/= Adjusted OR, confounding is likely If Crude OR = Adjusted OR, confounding is unlikely

Ideal “causal contrast” between exposed and unexposed groups: “A causal contrast compares disease frequency under two exposure distributions, but in one target population during one etiologic time period” If the ideal causal contrast is met, the observed effect is the “causal effect” Maldonado & Greenland, Int J Epi 2002;31:422-29

Ideal counterfactual comparison to determine causal effects: Exposed cohort Iexp Initial conditions are identical in the exposed and unexposed groups, except for presence of exposure (=cause) Unexposed cohort Iunexp RRcausal = Iexp / Iunexp Maldonado & Greenland, Int J Epi 2002;31:422-29

Isubstitute Iexp Iunexp RRassoc = Iexp / Isubstitute What happens in reality? Exposed cohort Iexp Unexposed cohort Iunexp Substitute, unexposed cohort Isubstitute RRassoc = Iexp / Isubstitute

RRcausal = Iexp / Iunexp In this case: RRcausal = Iexp / Iunexp IDEAL RRassoc = Iexp / Isubstitute ACTUAL “Confounding is present if the substitute population represents imperfectly what the target would have been like under the counterfactual condition”

Untreated individuals Simulating the counter-factual comparison: Experimental Studies: Randomized Clinical Trials Disease + Treated individuals Disease - Randomization Eligible population compare rates Disease + Untreated individuals Disease - Randomization helps to make the groups “comparable” (i.e. similar initial conditions) with respect to known and unknown confounders Confounding is unlikely at randomization - time t0

Simulating the counter-factual comparison: Observational Studies: Cohort studies, case-control studies Disease + Exposed cohort Disease - compare rates Disease + Unexposed cohort Disease - PRESENT FUTURE In observational studies, because exposures are not assigned randomly, attainment of exchangeability is impossible – “initial conditions” are likely to be different and the groups may not be comparable

Confounding: Observational studies vs randomized trials Example: Aspirin to reduce cardiovascular mortality

Confounding: adjustment and controls Control at the design stage Randomization Restriction Matching Control at the analysis stage Conventional approaches Stratified analyses Multivariate analyses Newer approaches Graphical approaches using DAGs Propensity scores Instrumental variables Marginal structural models 29

Options at the design stage: Randomization Reduces potential for confounding by generating groups that are fairly comparable with respect to known and unknown confounding variables Restriction Eliminates variation in the confounder (e.g. only recruiting one gender) Matching Involves selection of a comparison group that is forced to resemble the index group with respect to the distribution of one or more potential confounders 30

Randomization Randomization Only for intervention studies Definition: random assignment of study subjects to exposure categories To control/reduce the effect of confounding variables about which the investigator is unaware (i.e. both known and unknown confounders get distributed evenly because of randomization) Randomization does not always eliminate confounding Covariate imbalance in small trials “Maldistribution” of potentially confounding variables after randomization (“Table I: Baseline characteristics” in the randomized trial)

X C D E Randomization breaks any links between treatment and prognostic factors Confounder C Randomization X Exposure Disease (outcome) D E 32

Restriction The distribution of the potential confounding factors does not vary across exposure or disease categories An investigator may restrict study subjects to only those falling with specific level(s) of a confounding variable Advantages of restriction straightforward, convenient, inexpensive (but, reduces recruitment!) Disadvantages of restriction Limits number of eligible subjects Limits ability to generalize the study findings Residual confounding Impossible to evaluate the relationship of interest at different levels of the confounder

Matching Matching is commonly used in case-control studies Match on strong confounder Types: Pair (individual) matching Frequency matching The use of matching usually requires special analysis techniques (e.g. matched pair analyses and conditional logistic regression)

Matching Disadvantages of matching Finding appropriate control subjects: difficult and expensive and limit sample size Confounder used to match subjects cannot be evaluated with respect to the outcome/disease Matching does not control for confounders other than those used to match The use of matching makes the use of stratified analysis very difficult Matching is most often used in case-control studies (prohibitive in a large cohort study) In a case-control study, matching may even introduce confounding

Controlling Confounding: At the analysis stage Conventional approaches

Confounding: control at the analysis stage Confounding is one type of bias that can be adjusted in the analysis (unlike selection and information bias) Options at the analysis stage: Stratification Multivariate methods To control for confounding in the analyses, confounders must be measured in the study 37

Stratification Produce groups within which the confounder does not vary Evaluate the exposure-disease association within each stratum of the confounder 38

Source: www.epiet.org 39

Stratified Analysis Crude Crude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify by Confounder Calculate OR’s for each stratum If stratum-specific OR’s are similar, calculate adjusted OR (e.g. MH) ORCrude Stratum 1 Stratum 2 OR1 OR2 If Crude OR =/= Adjusted OR, confounding is likely If Crude OR = Adjusted OR, confounding is unlikely

Direction of Confounding Confounding “pulls” the observed association away from the true association It can either exaggerate/over-estimate the true association (positive confounding) Example ORcausal = 1.0 ORobserved = 3.0 or It can hide/under-estimate the true association (negative confounding) ORcausal = 3.0 ORobserved = 1.0 41

Multivariate Analysis Stratified analysis works best only in the presence of 1 or 2 confounders If the number of potential confounders is large, multivariate analyses offer the only real solution Can handle large numbers of confounders (covariates) simultaneously Based on statistical regression “models” E.g. logistic regression, multiple linear regression Always done with statistical software packages 42

Residual confounding Confounding that can persist, even after adjustment Unmeasured confounding Some variables were actually not confounders Confounders were measured with error (eg., misclassification) Categories of the confounder improperly defined 43

Effect modification and interaction Maura Pugliatti, MD, PhD Associate Professor of Neurology Dept. of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Unit of Clinical Neurology University of Sassari, Italy 1st International Course of Neuroepidemiology Chisinau, Moldova, 24-28 Sept. 2012 45

Definition Biological interaction Effect modification (“effect-measure modification”) Heterogeneity of effects Subgroup effects Statistical Interaction Deviation from a specified model form (additive or multiplicative)

Biological interaction “the interdependent operation of two or more biological causes to produce, prevent or control an effect” [Porta, Dictionary, 2008]

Multicausality and interdependent effects Disease processes tend to be multifactorial: “multicausality” The “one-variable-at-a-time” perspective has several limitations Confounding and effect modification: manifestations of multicausality Schoenbach, 2000

Effect modification and statistical interaction Two definitions (related): Based on homogeneity or heterogeneity of effects Interaction occurs when the effect of a risk factor (X) on an outcome (Y) is not homogeneous in strata formed by a third variable (Z, effect modifier) “Differences in the effect measure for one factor at different levels of another factor” [Porta, 2008] This is often called “effect modification” Based on the comparison between observed and expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable Interaction occurs when the observed joint effects of the risk factor (X) and third variable (Z) differs from that expected on the basis of their independent effects This is often called “statistical interaction” [deviation from some specified model] Szklo & Nieto, Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. 2007

Definition based on homogeneity or heterogeneity of effects Effect of exposure on the disease is modified depending on the value of a third variable: the “effect modifier” Effect modifier Exposure Disease

Stratified Analysis Crude Crude 2 x 2 table Calculate Crude OR (or RR) Stratify by Confounder Calculate OR’s for each stratum ORCrude Stratum 1 Stratum 2 OR1 OR2 If stratum-specific OR’s are the same or similar, calculate adjusted OR (e.g. MH) If stratum-specific OR’s are not similar, calculate adjusted OR (e.g. MH) Effect modification is present. Report Stratum-specific OR If Crude OR =/= Adjusted OR, confounding is likely. Report Adjusted OR If Crude OR = Adjusted OR, confounding is unlikely. Report Crude OR

Confounding vs. interaction Confounding is a problem we want to eliminate (control or adjust for) in our study Comparing crude vs. adjusted effect estimates Interaction is a natural occurrence that we want to describe and study further Comparing stratum-specific estimates

Heterogeneity of effects Can occur at the level of: Individual study: within subgroups of a single study or trial Seen in subgroup or stratified analyses within a study Across studies: if several studies are done on the same topic, the effect measures may vary across studies Seen in meta-analyses (across trials)

Definition based on the comparison between observed and expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable Deviation from additive or multiplicative joint effects This is often called “statistical interaction”

Szklo & Nieto, Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. 2007 Observed vs expected joint effects of a risk factor and a third variable No interaction Positive interaction Negative interaction Szklo & Nieto, Epidemiology: Beyond the basics. 2007

Deviation from additive or multiplicative joint effects Interaction on an “additive” scale (additive interaction) Effect measure modification when risk difference is used as measure of effect Additive statistical model: Linear regression: y = a + b1x1 + b2x2 Interaction on a “multiplicative” scale (multiplicative interaction) Effect measure modification when risk ratio is used as measure of effect Multiplicative statistical model: Logistic regression:

Additive or multiplicative model? The additive model underpins the methods for assessing biological interaction Interaction here is a departure from additivity of disease rates (risk difference is the key measure) Risk difference scale is of greatest public health importance (based on attributable risk) Many of the models used in epidemiology are inherently multiplicative (e.g. logistic regression) Vast majority of epi analyses implicitly use the multiplicative scale (risk ratio is the key measure) Because most epi studies report RR and OR estimates and use regression models such as logistic and survival analyses – these models inherently use ratio measures and are therefore multiplicative Ahlbom A et al. Eur J Epi 2005

Why is interaction/effect modification important? Better understanding of causation Identification of “high-risk” groups Target interventions at specific subgroups