1 Psych 5500/6500 Confounding Variables Fall, 2008.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Agenda Group Hypotheses Validity of Inferences from Research Inferences and Errors Types of Validity Threats to Validity.
Advertisements

Making Inferences about Causality In general, children who watch violent television programs tend to behave more aggressively toward their peers and siblings.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon (2007) Single-Variable, Independent-Groups Designs Graziano and Raulin Research Methods: Chapter 10 This multimedia product and.
Psych 5500/6500 t Test for Two Independent Groups: Power Fall, 2008.
Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Research
1 COMM 301: Empirical Research in Communication Lecture 10 Kwan M Lee.
Between- vs. Within-Subjects Designs
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon (2010) Single-Variable, Independent-Groups Designs Graziano and Raulin Research Methods: Chapter 10 This multimedia product and.
Copyright © 2010, 2007, 2004 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies.
Analysis of frequency counts with Chi square
2.6 The Question of Causation. The goal in many studies is to establish a causal link between a change in the explanatory variable and a change in the.
Quasi-Experimental Designs
Who are the participants? Creating a Quality Sample 47:269: Research Methods I Dr. Leonard March 22, 2010.
Experimental Design: Single factor designs Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
Non-Experimental designs: Developmental designs & Small-N designs
Experimental Control & Design Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
47.269: Research I: The Basics Dr. Leonard Spring 2010
Non-Experimental designs: Developmental designs & Small-N designs
Experimental Control Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
Sampling & Experimental Control Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
Experimental Control & Design Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
Chapter 9 Experimental Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian
Smith/Davis (c) 2005 Prentice Hall Chapter Ten Designing and Conducting, Experiments with Two Groups PowerPoint Presentation created by Dr. Susan R. Burns.
Chapter 4 Hypothesis Testing, Power, and Control: A Review of the Basics.
Inferential Statistics & Test of Significance
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies.
Experiments and Observational Studies. Observational Studies In an observational study, researchers don’t assign choices; they simply observe them. look.
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Slide
Chapter 13 Observational Studies & Experimental Design.
Copyright © 2007 Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Pearson Addison-Wesley Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies.
Experiment Basics: Variables Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
COURSE: JUST 3900 INTRODUCTORY STATISTICS FOR CRIMINAL JUSTICE Instructor: Dr. John J. Kerbs, Associate Professor Joint Ph.D. in Social Work and Sociology.
Experimental Design making causal inferences Richard Lambert, Ph.D.
Slide 13-1 Copyright © 2004 Pearson Education, Inc.
1 Psych 5500/6500 Populations, Samples, Sampling Procedures, and Bias Fall, 2008.
Experimental Design All experiments have independent variables, dependent variables, and experimental units. Independent variable. An independent.
Causal inferences During the last two lectures we have been discussing ways to make inferences about the causal relationships between variables. One of.
1 Psych 5500/6500 t Test for Two Independent Means Fall, 2008.
Between groups designs (2) – outline 1.Block randomization 2.Natural groups designs 3.Subject loss 4.Some unsatisfactory alternatives to true experiments.
Inference and Inferential Statistics Methods of Educational Research EDU 660.
Chapter 3.1.  Observational Study: involves passive data collection (observe, record or measure but don’t interfere)  Experiment: ~Involves active data.
1 Psych 5500/6500 t Test for Dependent Groups (aka ‘Paired Samples’ Design) Fall, 2008.
Educational Research Chapter 13 Inferential Statistics Gay, Mills, and Airasian 10 th Edition.
Introduction section of article
1.) *Experiment* 2.) Quasi-Experiment 3.) Correlation 4.) Naturalistic Observation 5.) Case Study 6.) Survey Research.
Chapter 10 Experimental Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian 10th Edition
CHAPTER 2 Research Methods in Industrial/Organizational Psychology
1 Psych 5510/6510 Chapter 13 ANCOVA: Models with Continuous and Categorical Predictors Part 2: Controlling for Confounding Variables Spring, 2009.
Chapter 8: Between Subjects Designs
Aim: What factors must we consider to make an experimental design?
The Influence of Suggestion on Subjective Preferences By Sean Oh, Joshua Marcuse, and David Atterbury Math 5: Chance.
Experimental and Ex Post Facto Designs
Causal inferences This week we have been discussing ways to make inferences about the causal relationships between variables. One of the strongest ways.
Producing Data: Experiments BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 9 1.
Making Inferences about Causality In general, children who watch violent television programs tend to behave more aggressively toward their peers and siblings.
Quazi-Experimental Designs. Outline of Today’s Discussion 1.True Experiments 2.Quasi-experimental Designs.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies.
Experimental Design-Chapter 8
Controlling Contaminating or “Nuisance” Variables
CHAPTER 2 Research Methods in Industrial/Organizational Psychology
Single-Variable, Independent-Groups Designs
2 independent Groups Graziano & Raulin (1997).
Designing Experiments
Experiments with Two Groups
Establishing the Direction of the Relationship
Experimental Design: The Basic Building Blocks
Inferential Statistics & Test of Significance
Experiment Basics: Variables
Reminder for next week CUELT Conference.
Chapter Ten: Designing, Conducting, Analyzing, and Interpreting Experiments with Two Groups The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis.
Presentation transcript:

1 Psych 5500/6500 Confounding Variables Fall, 2008

2 Confounding Variables We are running a t test in an attempt to establish a cause and effect relationship between the independent and dependent variables. If we reject H0, we are saying that the difference in the group means is statistically significant, i.e. that the difference is larger than would be expected if only random error were involved. We would then like to conclude that it was the independent variable that made the groups means different. We can only conclude this, however, if we have no confounding variables in our experiment.

3 Confounding (cont.) A confounding variable is a variable other than our independent variable (and other than random chance) that could have caused the group means to be different. For example: I.V.=gender, D.V.=respiration rate. We run the experiment and find that the difference between the two groups is statistically significant. We would like to conclude that gender affects respiration rate. But what if we measured the men in the morning and the women in the evening, then ‘time of day’ could account for the significant difference between the groups, ‘time of day’ would be a ‘confounding variable’.

4 Confounding (cont.) If we have a confounding variable, and we reject H0, then we do not know whether it was our independent variable or the confounding variable that caused the group means to differ. If we do not have a serious confounding variable then when we reject H0 we can conclude that it was our independent variable that caused the group means to differ

5 Handling Confounding Variables Handling confounding variables is an important topic in experimental design, the important point in a statistics class is to realize that rejecting H0 only indicates that the difference between the means was unlikely if only chance were involved. To then conclude that the independent variable had an effect is only reasonable if there are no serious confounding variables. Even though this is more a topic for an experimental design class, I think it would behoove us to take a quick look at how to handle confounding variables. We’ll look at four possibilities:

6 Handling Confounding Variables 1)Pretest to see if there is a problem: for example, before you run your experiment you could run a quick pretest to see if time of day influences respiration, if you find it does not then don’t worry about time of day being a confounding variable in your experiment.

7 Handling Confounding Variables 2)Control the variable: if time of day is a confounding variable then design the study so that it effects both groups equally, in this way you are controlling the confounding variable. For example, run both groups at the same time, or test one male and one female at 8:00 in the morning, one male and one female at 8:30 and so on.

8 Control and Treatment Groups Often the experimenter wants to examine the effect of some manipulation (treatment). To do so two groups are created, the ‘control’ group that does not get the treatment and the ‘treatment’ group that does. The goal is to control all of the possible serious confounding variables, so that the only difference between the control group and the treatment group is the treatment itself.

9 Handling Confounding Variables 3)Randomize the variable: sometimes you may not be able to control the confounding variable. If this is the case, then arrange for its effect to be randomly divided among the participants. For example, randomly determine what time of day each participant is measured. The confounding variable might still effect the data (e.g. by luck more males may be measured in the morning than females) but at least its effect will be random, and will have an equal chance of contradicting your theory’s prediction as supporting it. Randomly assigning subjects to groups randomizes the effect of individual differences.

10 Handling Confounding Variables 4)Include the confounding variable in your model: there are a lot of advantages to looking at how both time of day and gender affect respiration (rather than trying to get rid of time of day so you can just look at gender). We will be covering this next semester when we get to the ‘Model Comparison Approach’.

11 Experimental Designs The t test for independent groups can be used in three different types of experimental designs. Some of these designs introduce additional confounding variables. 1.True Experimental Design 2.Quasi-Experimental Design 3.Static Group Design

12 True Experimental Design Attributes: 1.Subjects are randomly divided into groups. 2.The independent variable is manipulated by the experimenter If performed correctly, then the only reason the groups should differ before the IV is applied is because of random chance. This design may still have confounding variables (introduced by the how you manipulate the independent variable), but it tends to have fewer confounding variables than the other two designs.

13 Quasi-Experimental Design Attributes: 1.Subjects are not randomly divided into groups, they are in pre-existing groups that you hope are essentially the same (i.e. as if you had randomly divided into groups). 2.Independent variable manipulated by the experimenter. This design is used when you would like to do a true experimental design but practical reasons make that impossible. Additional confounding variables are introduced by possible pre-existing, non-random, differences between the groups. Possible solutions: search for groups that are equal, test for possible confounding variables.

14 Quasi-Experimental Design (cont.) For more information on quasi-experimental designs you may want to read the influential work of Campbell. You could start with: Campbell, Donald T. (1969). Reforms as experiments. American Psychologist, 24, In looking for what he has done recently I ran across the following (which I haven’t read) but looks good for light summer reading (seriously, looks good if you really want to get into the topic of establishing cause and effect with experiments). Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., Campbell, D. T. (2002) Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin and Company. 623 pp.

15 Static Group Design Attributes: 1.Subjects are not randomly divided into groups, the groups are pre-existing. 2.The independent variable is not manipulated by the experimenter, instead it is the variable you use to define the two groups. In this design you are explicitly looking for pre-existing differences between the groups (which is your I.V.). In the gender example the I.V. (gender) was what you used to divide participants into the two groups. Additional confounding variables are pre-existing differences other than your I.V. (i.e. other variables that systematically differ along with the I.V., such as height differences that occur when you divide participants based upon gender).

16 Static Group Design (cont.) The static group design is considered to be a correlational design as the independent variable is not manipulated. It shows up here, however, rather than in the latter lecture on correlation as the focus here is on comparing the two group means, rather than upon a correlation between two variables.