Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Does the home base anchor spatial mapping? *D.G. Wallace 1 ; D.A. Hamilton 2 ; A.D. Choate 1 1 Department of Psychology,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The bead maze: Adapting the water maze task to the human manipulatory scale E.L. Loda, A.B. Thoennes, J.R. Köppen, S.S. Winter, D.A. Hamilton, D.G. Wallace.
Advertisements

192-IgG Saporin lesions of the medial septum or nucleus basalis magnocellularis disrupt exploratory trip organization. D.G. Wallace*, S.S. Winter, M.M.
Original Figures for "Molecular Classification of Cancer: Class Discovery and Class Prediction by Gene Expression Monitoring"
AMPHETAMINE AND HALOPERIDOL MAY ALTER TEMPORAL PROCESSING DURING A NATURALLY OCCURRING BEHAVIOR P.S.Wallace 1* ; E.F.Field 2 ; I.Q.Whishaw 2 ; D.G.Wallace.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective cholinergic lesions of the medial septum disrupt dead reckoning based navigation M.M. Martin*; M.D. Schultz; S.S.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Blindly Biased: Restricting Cue Access Impacts Spatial Orientation in a Human Analogue of the Water Maze B.P. Apger, D.M.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation disrupts exploratory trip organization D.G. Wallace*; S.K. Knapp; J.A. Silver;
Methods Results Conclusions Walking the plank: Role of the medial septum in distance estimation S.S. Winter; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology,
Humans were able to accurately use dead-reckoning to estimate distance and direction on a smaller scale. Participants modulated their peak speed on the.
A Human Analogue of Rat Food Hoarding Behavior: Searching Path Kinematics Influence the Accuracy of Dead Reckoning S.N. Choudhry; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace*
Conclusions Both groups set up a home base under novel-dark and light conditions and display a similar topographical organization of exploratory trips.
Unilateral lesions of the medial agranular cortex produce a persistent disruption in the organization of food protection behaviors S. J. WAGNER, M. J.
Methods Results Conclusions Mammillothalamic tract lesions disrupt self-movement cue processing in a food hoarding task. *S.S. Winter; J.L. McMillin, D.G.
Methods Results Conclusions Getting lost in the dark: Disruption of directional but not distance estimates following mammillothalamic tract lesions *S.S.
Navigating with fingers and feet: Comparative analysis of human and rat movement kinematics during non-visual spatial tasks J.R. Raines, N.D. McNeal, J.L.
Figure 4: Average dodger speeds are shown for MS (left panel) and NB (right panel) groups. Dodging (circles) and bracing (triangles) speeds are plotted.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Running on rungs: Serial pattern learning on the horizontal ladder *M.J Hylin 1 ; S.S. Winter 1 ; T. L. Marsiglio 1 ;
Figure 5: Initial (left panel) and terminal (right panel) distances between the noses of the dodger and robber rats are plotted separately for dodging.
Do Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Ever Return Home? S.L.Bennett 1* ; D.A.Hamilton 2 ; D.G.Wallace 1 1. Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb,
Neural mechanisms of Spatial Learning. Spatial Learning Materials covered in previous lectures Historical development –Tolman and cognitive maps the classic.
Figure 12: Each group’s average correlations are plotted for cued, uncued and dark probes. Group differences were observed during the uncued and dark probes.
Making sense of the senseless: Evaluating sex differences of ASO
NATURE NEUROSCIENCE 2007 Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep Daoyun Ji & Matthew A Wilson Department of Brain.
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Dmitriy Aronov, David W. Tank  Neuron 
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Neural Mechanisms of Self-Location
Volume 88, Issue 2, Pages (October 2015)
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Joshua P. Bassett, Thomas J. Wills, Francesca Cacucci  Current Biology 
Volume 14, Issue 11, Pages (March 2016)
The Mechanism of Rate Remapping in the Dentate Gyrus
Kiah Hardcastle, Surya Ganguli, Lisa M. Giocomo  Neuron 
Single Units in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex with Anxiety-Related Firing Patterns Are Preferentially Influenced by Ventral Hippocampal Activity  Avishek.
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Enhanced Depolymerization of Actin Filaments by ADF/Cofilin and Monomer Funneling by Capping Protein Cooperate to Accelerate Barbed-End Growth  Shashank.
Volume 72, Issue 4, Pages (November 2011)
Annabelle C. Singer, Loren M. Frank  Neuron 
Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Guifen Chen, Daniel Manson, Francesca Cacucci, Thomas Joseph Wills 
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
Georg B. Keller, Tobias Bonhoeffer, Mark Hübener  Neuron 
What do grid cells contribute to place cell firing?
Pattern Selection by Dynamical Biochemical Signals
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Intracellular Encoding of Spatiotemporal Guidance Cues in a Self-Organizing Signaling System for Chemotaxis in Dictyostelium Cells  Tatsuo Shibata, Masatoshi.
A Quarter of a Century of Place Cells
Volume 22, Issue 12, Pages (March 2018)
Volume 100, Issue 11, Pages (June 2011)
Adrienn G. Varga, Roy E. Ritzmann  Current Biology 
Synchronized Activity between the Ventral Hippocampus and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Anxiety  Avishek Adhikari, Mihir A. Topiwala, Joshua A.
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (September 2000)
Traces of Experience in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
Volume 28, Issue 8, Pages e3 (April 2018)
Volume 24, Issue 8, Pages e6 (August 2018)
Passive Transport Disrupts Grid Signals in the Parahippocampal Cortex
Volume 22, Issue 12, Pages (March 2018)
End-Stopping and the Aperture Problem
The Hippocampus, Memory, and Place Cells
Honghui Zhang, Andrew J. Watrous, Ansh Patel, Joshua Jacobs  Neuron 
How Cells Tiptoe on Adhesive Surfaces before Sticking
Steven C. Leiser, Karen A. Moxon  Neuron 
Locomotor and Hippocampal Processing Converge in the Lateral Septum
Volume 37, Issue 3, Pages (February 2003)
Presentation transcript:

Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Does the home base anchor spatial mapping? *D.G. Wallace 1 ; D.A. Hamilton 2 ; A.D. Choate 1 1 Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, De Kalb, IL, USA 2 Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA Rat exploratory behavior in an open field is organized around a single location, or home base. The home base appears to anchor spatial exploration in that animals spend a great deal of time there and return to the home base after exploratory trips. Recent work has suggested that both exteroceptive visual (allothetic) cues and self-movement cues contribute to this organization. Under normal light conditions, salient allothetic cues exhibit control over the location of the home base. A home base is also established under dark conditions suggesting that self-movement cues contribute to the organization of exploratory behavior. Considering that both place cells and head direction cells exhibit parallel response characteristics to manipulations of allothetic cues, and that place field firing rate maps established under light conditions are maintained in the dark by self- movement information, the home base may anchor the representation of relationships between allothetic and self- movement navigational systems. Whereas previous studies of home base behavior have utilized large table-tops, the current study examined home base formation and the control of the home base location by allothetic cues in an apparatus traditionally used in place cell and head direction cell studies. Rats were disoriented and placed in a transparent Plexiglas cylinder (91.5 cm) in which stimulus characteristics of the wall could be manipulated by wrapping different segments with black, white, or gray poster board. The animals were free to explore the cylinder for 60 minutes during each daily session. Figure 1: Diagram of the apparatus used in the current study. Location and visual characteristics of the cue card can be manipulated without altering the cylinder. Home base establishment and stability was examined in a cylinder with black walls and a white cue card. During the first 60 minute session the cue card was in the same position for all rats (n=21). On the second day, the cue remained in the same position for the rats in the stable group (n=9) and was rotated 90o for rats in the rotate group (n=12). Figure 2: A representative rat’s total time (A), cumulative change in heading (B), rearing frequency (C), and inverse of the mean linear speeds (D) are plotted for each 22.5o zone from a single session. Home base establishment is a ubiquitous characteristic of rat behavior in traditional place cell apparati. Visual and spatial characteristics of the cylinder influence home base location. Highly salient visual cues polarize home base location. Less salient visual cues weakly control home base location. This work is consistent with a role for the home base in anchoring a spatial mapping system to an idiothetic cue based system. 66.9/Z26 Introduction N : 180 o S : 0 o W : 270 o E : 270 o A.B. C. D. Figure 3: A representative rat’s path and distribution of time spent in different regions of the cylinder are plotted for one session (left). The mean parameter of concentration for the cumulative time measure is plotted for each group across both sessions (right). Figure 4: Average correlations between cumulative time and change in heading (left), rearing (middle), and inverse of speed (right) are plotted for each group across both days. Figure 5: Polar graphs plot home base location for stable (left) and rotate (right) groups across both days (Day 1: blue; Day 2: red). Clockwise (dotted lines) and counter-clockwise rotations (solid lines) are indicated for each rat. The bottom panel plots the mean difference in heading direction of home base for each group. This experiment examined the effects of manipulating visual characteristics of the cylinder wall on home base location. Each group of rats explored a cylinder for four 60-minute sessions. During the first three sessions the cylinder remained stable. On the forth day, the cylinder was rotated 90o. Figure 6: Illustrations provide a schematic of the cylinders explored by the Black-White (left), Gray-White (middle), and White-Black (right) groups. Experiment 2 Figure 7: Parameter of concentration for the cumulative time measure are plotted for each group across the four daily sessions (A). Average correlations between cumulative time and change in heading (B), rearing (C), and inverse of speed (D) are plotted for each group. A.B. C. D. Figure 8: Home base locations are plotted for each rat in the black- white (left), gray-white (middle), and white-black (right) groups. The background of each graph represents the visual and spatial characteristics of the cylinder across sessions. Figure 9: The second order circular statistics were used to examine whether home base locations were random for each group across sessions. (Rayleigh uniform distribution test, *p<.05). Acknowledgments: We would like to thank Megan Martin for her assistance in data collection related to the current project. Correspondence should be addressed to Douglas G. Wallace: or