Nonword Repetition and Sentence Repetition as Clinical Markers of SLI: The Case of Cantonese Stokes, F. S., Wong, M.Y.A., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, B. L.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Method Participants 184 five-year-old (M age=5.63, SD=0.22) kindergarten students from 30 classrooms in central Illinois Teacher ratings The second edition.
Advertisements

> Main questions of the study: (1)Are there global differences in reading speed and accuracy between dyslexics and controls across.
Tone perception and production by Cantonese-speaking and English- speaking L2 learners of Mandarin Chinese Yen-Chen Hao Indiana University.
Contrastive Analysis, Error Analysis, Interlanguage
Development of a Non-pharmacological Model of Cognitive Impairment for the Evaluation of Putative Pro-cognitive Agents – Potential Relevance for Age-Related.
Examining the Relationship Between Confrontational Naming Tasks & Discourse Production in Aphasia Leila D. Luna & Gerasimos Fergadiotis Portland State.
Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination Jessica Maye, Janet F. Werker, LouAnn Gerken A brief article from Cognition.
Passive comprehension by Slovak typically and atypically developing children Master thesis in psychology, Author: Radka Antalíková, Supervisor: Kristine.
Measuring Referring Expressions in a Story Context Phyllis Schneider, Speech Pathology & Audiology, University of Alberta Denyse Hayward, University of.
Aimee L. Arnoldussen; Julia L. Evans; Mark S. Seidenberg Performance Matching: Comparing Children with Specific Language Impairment to Younger Children.
Handwriting performance of children with dyslexia
For cases of severe pediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI), age at injury is positively correlated with speech outcome after 12 months as measured by Percentage.
Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey
Sentence Repetition Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.
Perception of syllable prominence by listeners with and without competence in the tested language Anders Eriksson 1, Esther Grabe 2 & Hartmut Traunmüller.
Memory Span and Narrative Skills – Where’s the Connection? Line Engel Clasen, Kristine Jensen de López & Hanne Bruun Søndergaard Knudsen University of.
Inductive reasoning and implicit memory: evidence from intact and impaired memory systems Authors: Luisa Girelli, Carlo Semenza and Margarete Delazer.
Profile of Phoneme Auditory Perception Ability in Children with Hearing Impairment and Phonological Disorders By Manal Mohamed El-Banna (MD) Unit of Phoniatrics,
Non-Word Repetition Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.
T-test. Tests ParametricNon-parametric between / independent / unrelated Independent t- test Mann-Whitney U within / dependent / related / repeated measures.
Signed Paired Associates Test (SPAT). SPAT Structure Similar to WMS “paired associates” subtest 14 sign pairs – 7 easy & 7 hard Based on sign associate.
The Effects of Increased Cognitive Demands on the Written Discourse Ability of Young Adolescents Ashleigh Elaine Zumwalt Eastern Illinois University.
Second Language Proficiency Places Cognitive Constraints on Sentence Processing Noriko Hoshino Department of Psychology The Pennsylvania State University.
Verb inflections as indicators of Bilingual SLI Sharon Armon-Lotem, The Bilingual SLI project* Bar Ilan University *This project is funded by ISF grant.
Review of three tests of children’s narrative ability [Poster presented at Narratives, Intervention, and Literacy conference, Paris, France, Sept. 2012]
Smith/Davis (c) 2005 Prentice Hall Chapter Eight Correlation and Prediction PowerPoint Presentation created by Dr. Susan R. Burns Morningside College.
Figurative Language Development Research and Popular Children’s Literature: Why We Should Know, “Where the Wild Things Are” Kathleen Ahrens.
Review of the paper entitled “The development of a phonetically balanced word recognition test in the Ilocano language” written by Renita Sagon, Doctor.
CSD 2230 HUMAN COMMUNICATION DISORDERS
Implication of Gender and Perception of Self- Competence on Educational Aspiration among Graduates in Taiwan Wan-Chen Hsu and Chia- Hsun Chiang Presenter.
1 ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN TYPICALLY DEVELOPING CHILDREN’S COMPLEX SENTENCE COMPREHENSION AUTHORS; Shwetha M.P.,Deepthi M. Trupthi T, Nikhil Mathur &
N ON WORD REPETITION OF TARGET AND NON TARGET LIKE WORDS [ BY MONOLINGUAL A RABIC PRESCHOOLERS ] Instructor: Dr. Sharon Armon Lotem This long paper was.
X Language Acquisition
The fragile-X syndrome: What about the deficit in the pragmatic component of language? Abstract The fragile-X syndrome: What about the deficit in the pragmatic.
Understanding Statistics
Speech and Language Issues For Babies and Pre-school age children who have Down Syndrome Ups and Downs Southwest Conference 2007.
Does Phonological Awareness Intervention Impact Speech Production in a 3-year-old? Kayla Knueppel, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders Vicki.
Objective The current study examined whether the timing of recovery from late onset of productive vocabulary (e.g., either earlier or later blooming) was.
Specific Language Impairment & Cognition: A Meta-Analysis Michael W. Casby Communicative Sciences & Disorders Michigan State University imail:
1 Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception Sandra Anacleto uOttawa.
Presented by: Odelya Ohana. Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989 NWR phonological short-term memory. Gathercole, 2006 Phonological storage is the key capacity.
The Critical Period for Language Acquisition: Evidence from Second Language Learning CATHERINE E. SNOW AND MARIAN HOEFNAGEL-HÖHLE UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM.
Introduction The impact of Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) on cognitive and language abilities of individuals with Parkinson’s disease.
Experimental Research Methods in Language Learning Chapter 12 Reliability and Reliability Analysis.
Language Intervention and the Effect on Passive Comprehension – A pilot-study Lone Sundahl Olsen, ph.D. Student, University of Aalborg, Denmark Master.
Aimee L. Arnoldussen 1 ; Julia L. Evans 2 ; Mark S. Seidenberg 1,3 Neuroscience Training Program 1 ; Department of Communicative Disorders 2 ; Department.
Early Identification of Introductory Major's Biology Students for Inclusion in an Academic Support Program BETHANY V. BOWLING and E. DAVID THOMPSON Department.
Working Memory and Language Learning: A Longitudinal Study of Trilingual Children Pascale Engel Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford.
Motion Perception Deficits and Reading Impairment It’s the noise, not the motion A. Sperling, Z-L. Lu, F. Manis & M. Seidenberg.
Heilmann, J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J. and C. Hollar. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventory in Identifying Language.
Reinforcement Look at matched picture after sound ends & it moves 10 trials (5 of each pairing) 2 or 4 blocks (2 pairs of words, 2 pairs of swoops) Participants.
Processing Faces with Emotional Expressions: Negative Faces Cause Greater Stroop Interference for Young and Older Adults Gabrielle Osborne 1, Deborah Burke.
By Dakota Dye Mentor: Melissa Allen. Misdiagnosis and overrepresentation of minority children in special education. Clinicians struggle to determine whether.
1 Vocabulary acquisition from extensive reading: A case study Maria Pigada and Norbert Schmitt ( 2006)
The influence of forgetting rate on complex span and academic performance Debbora Hall, Chris Jarrold, John Towse and Amy Zarandi.
Does the brain compute confidence estimates about decisions?
In the Age of Common Core. Close Reading of text involves an investigation of a short piece of text, with multiple readings done over multiple instructional.
Child Syntax and Morphology
Late talkers (Delayed Onset)
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska
2nd Language Learning Chapter 2 Lecture 4.
Evaluating the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis in Preschool Children
Copyright © American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
This research was funded by a small grant from the EPS society.
Susan Geffen, Suzanne Curtin and Susan Graham
Which of these is “a boy”?
Cognitive flexibility in children with specific language impairment (SLI) Contact Information: Klara Marton Barczi College,
A perceptual investigation of prosodic accuracy in children with typical language and specific language impairment Peter Richtsmeier
Noriko Hoshino Department of Psychology
Roger Brown’s (1973) First Language Development Study and MLU
Presentation transcript:

Nonword Repetition and Sentence Repetition as Clinical Markers of SLI: The Case of Cantonese Stokes, F. S., Wong, M.Y.A., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, B. L. (2006). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49,

Research has shown that English speaking children with SLI score lower than their age-matched typically developing peers (TDAM) and language matched typically developing peers (TDLM), on tests of working memory. For this reason research has focused on both: 1. Phonological working memory via tasks of nonword repetition (NWR) and sentence repetition (SR) 2. Functional working memory Purpose: Recent research suggests that NWR & SR tasks can be used as clinical markers to discriminate between children with SLI and their TDAM &TDY peers.

Although children with SLI scored significantly lower than their TDAM and TDY peers on NWR tasks it is not known what underpins the difference. It is claimed that working memory deficits underlie the poor performance of children with SLI, but research has shown that there are more factors to consider: 1. The lexicality effects of NWR stimuli 2. The phonological structure of the language under investigation 3. prosodic influences The researchers in this article sought to explore the issues of prosodic structure and lexicality effects by investigation of NWR in Cantonese, where prosodic structure is not an issue.

SR tasks have long been part of a screening battery for the identification of language impairment. The authors suggested that the source of the difficulties on SR could be either general working memory deficits or “deficit in linguistic processing”. Conti-Ramsden at al., (2001) said that SR must tap a child’s knowledge, whereas NWR does not. If Cantonese speaking children with SLI have no difficulty with tests of NWR & SR then it is possible that the differences found in previous studies between TDAM & SLI groups may be due to differences in language ability rather than in working memory.

Means and between-group differences on the language measures F (2,41)SLITDYTDAM (14)(15) N (=44) b.17 a CRDLS-R b 3.1 a CRVT 8.37 b.99 a MLU 1. CRDLS-R: The Cantonese version of the Reynell Developmental Language Scales. 2. CVRT: The Hong Kong Cantonese receptive vocabulary test. 3. MLU: The mean length of utterance.

24 nonwords were constructed for this study 6 for each of the 4 syllable lengths The syllables selected for the nonwords do not occur in the Cantonese language. Four syllable lengths: CVC, CVCVC, CVCVCVC, & CVCVCVCVC The selection process that the researchers used resulted in: IN syllables (CV combinations that occur in Cantonese syllabary) & OUT syllables (CV combinations that did not occur in Cantonese). Task: NWR of IN nonwords and OUT nonwords were compared.

SR performance was compared using 2 sentence types and 4 different scoring methods; The 2 sentence types were either 9 or 10 syllables in length: 1. Aspect-marker - sentences contained the elements of subject, verb, aspect marker, and object; e.g: go3 naam4 zai2 hou2 daai6lik6 caat3 gan2 deoi3 haai4 (The boy is polishing the shoes very hard. ) 2. The Passive - contained the passive elements. e.g: Ceong4 tau4faat3 go3 naam4zai2bei2 zek3 gau2 zeoi1 (The boy with long hair was chased by the dog.)

Four methods of scoring for the SR task: ) 8 sentences for each sentence type) 1. Complete sentence correct: The participant scored a point for each sentence repeated correctly, with the inclusion of all sentence elements. 2. Core elements correct: A point was given if the child's repetition of the sentence included all of the core elements of both sentence types. Core Elements: Aspect: verb + aspect marker + noun Passive: be + agent + verb 3. Error method: was used to equate the researcher's results with those for English (Conti- Ramsden et al., 2001). The less errors the child made, the more points he received. (0-3 points). 4. Percentage syllables correct method: The calculation of the percentage of correct syllables, i.e. one point was given for each correct syllable.

Both the NWR and SR tasks were randomized. The NWR task: The Goal: to repeat the word exactly as it was heard. Trial items: 2 words and 2 nonwords. The test items were presented in order of increasing difficulty: one syllable nonwords followed by two syllables nonwords, etc. The SR task: The Goal: to repeat the sentence exactly as it was heard. There were three trial items. In the case of both tasks the trial items were repeated once in case of an incorrect response but no experimental items were repeated. Both tasks were administrated via free-field speakers and all responses were recorded.

Group1 syllable2 syllables3 syllables4 syllablesTotal % Correct TDAM TDY SLI Total Significant differences were found among the groups on total % correct. 2. The test of NWR was age sensitive, but did not distinguish between the children with SLI and their age-matched peers. Means in percentage for all syllable lengths on the non-word test for the participant groups.

One syllable IN One syllable OUT Two syllable IN Two syllable OUT Three syllable IN Three syllable OUT Four syllable IN Four syllable OUT CV structure TDAM TDY SLI Results: 1. The TDAM and the SLI group scored significantly higher than the TDY group on total NWR, on repetition of IN non-words and repetition of OUT non-words. 2. There were no significant differences between the TDAM and the SLI groups.

Scoring Method: Sentence Type:TDAM:TDY:SLI: Complete sentence correct Passives Aspect Core elements correct Passives Aspect Error scoringPassives Aspect % of syllables correct Passives Aspect Means for percentage of accuracy of SR for the three participant groups.

ERROR METHOD: Test ResultsSLITDAM Positive 110 Negative 315 Classification of children with SLI and their TDAM peers as typically developing or language impaired on a SR task. % OF SYLLABLES CORRECT METHOD: Test ResultsSLITDAM Positive 60 Negative 815 We can conclude that SR composed of passive and aspect sentences and scored by the error method may be a viable clinical marker of Cantonese SLI.

NWR Task: Both the SLI and TDAM groups scored higher than the TDY group. no difference between the SLI group and the TDAM group The two groups also scored the same on OUT nonwords. SR Task: On the SR task, the TDAM group scored higher than the SLI and TDY groups, who did not differ. Methods of scoring SR: the best measure was the Error Method which successfully distinguished between TDAM and SLI groups and should be further evaluated as a clinical marker of SLI in Cantonese! No correlation was found between performance on NWR and performance on SR In this study. If children with SLI were shown, across languages to have deficits in NWR and/or SR, then we would be one step closer to uncovering the cognitive mechanisms that contribute to language impairments.

Research with English-speaking children has shown a significant difference between children with SLI and their TDAM &TDY peers on tests of NWR. This poorer performance has been attributed to a: language-independent working memory constraint. We expect, then, that a working memory constraint, might be identified in other groups of children with SLI, regardless of the language. However, although NWR is a sensitive developmental test in Cantonese with older children scoring higher than younger children, there is no difference in performance between children with SLI and their TDAM peers. This may suggest that a working memory capacity limitation of the type believed to underlie NWR skills does not underpin language impairment in Cantonese speaking children.

Given the range of scoring methods used for SR tasks across studies, it was important to compare the effects of using one method over another. Awarding points according to the number of errors per sentence proved to be the most effective method, as it successfully differentiated between children with SLI and their TDAM peers. It is also important to consider the types of sentences chosen for the test: Application of two sentence types (aspect and passive) together proved to be the best identifier of SLI in this cohort while ensuring sufficient sentence complexity to avoid ceiling performance in the TDAM group. SR has the potential to be a useful clinical marker of SLI in Cantonese, as it is in English. Further work might explore prosodic and processing factors in cross-linguistic studies.

According to Vance and Plante (1994), it was suggested that measures that have discriminated accuracy above 90% were considered: good discriminators and those measures that have above 80% were considered: fair discriminators. The recommended accuracy should be above 80%. Sensitivity for the best method used in SR task was only 77%, and thus below the 80% recommended. Field testing with a larger sample is necessary!