Neural Correlates of Variations in Event Processing during Learning in Basolateral Amygdala Matthew R. Roesch*, Donna J. Calu, Guillem R. Esber, and Geoffrey.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning
Advertisements

Gene-gene interaction of COMT and DRD2 modulates context updating and novelty processing Manuel Garcia-Garcia, Francisco Barceló, Iria SanMiguel, Immaculada.
Visual Attention Attention is the ability to select objects of interest from the surrounding environment A reliable measure of attention is eye movement.
Electrophysiology of Visual Attention. Moran and Desimone (1985) “Classical” RF prediction: there should be no difference in responses in these two conditions.
Test Oct. 21 Review Session Oct 19 2pm in TH201 (that’s here)
Attention as Information Selection. Early Selection Early Selection model postulated that attention acted as a strict gate at the lowest levels of sensory.
Lecture 18&19: Stimulus Control (Pavlovian & Instrumental) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
Lecture 21: Avoidance Learning & Punishment Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
The auditory cortex mediates the perceptual effects of acoustic temporal expectation Santiago Jaramillo & Anthony M Zador Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory,
Abstract Cognitive control processes reduce the effects of irrelevant or misleading information on performance. We report a study suggesting that effective.
Introduction: What does phasic Dopamine encode ? With asymmetric coding of errors, the mean TD error at the time of reward is proportional to p(1-p) ->
Reward processing (1) There exists plenty of evidence that midbrain dopamine systems encode errors in reward predictions (Schultz, Neuron, 2002) Changes.
Consequences of Attentional Selection Single unit recordings.
 The misinformation effect refers to incorrect recall or source attribution of an item presented after a to-be-remembered event as having been presented.
Distinguishing Evidence Accumulation from Response Bias in Categorical Decision-Making Vincent P. Ferrera 1,2, Jack Grinband 1,2, Quan Xiao 1,2, Joy Hirsch.
Experimental Design Tali Sharot & Christian Kaul With slides taken from presentations by: Tor Wager Christian Ruff.
Arousal.
Zicong Zhang Authors Wendy A. Suzuki Professor of Neural Science and Psychology, New York University Research interest: Organization of memory.
Block Types: Pure blocks of singleton search or feature search, plus mixed blocks of singleton search and feature search. Predictions Singleton Search:
A vanishing sex difference Effects of environmental contingencies in a virtual Morris water task on male and female spatial navigation Mühl, Griego, Kabisch,
Neural mechanisms of Spatial Learning. Spatial Learning Materials covered in previous lectures Historical development –Tolman and cognitive maps the classic.
Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology Weill Medical College of Cornell University.
Localization of Auditory Stimulus in the Presence of an Auditory Cue By Albert Ler.
Disk Failures Eli Alshan. Agenda Articles survey – Failure Trends in a Large Disk Drive Population – Article review – Conclusions – Criticism – Disk failure.
Blocking The phenomenon of blocking tells us that what happens to one CS depends not only on its relationship to the US but also on the strength of other.
Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Nonsocial Events Across Infancy: Effects of Unimodal versus Bimodal Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Extinction of Conditioned Behavior Chapter 9 Effects of Extinction Extinction and Original Learning What is learned during Extinction.
Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons Masayuki Matsumoto & Okihide Hikosaka (2007) Presented by Miranda Stewart.
Neural correlates of risk sensitivity An fMRI study of instrumental choice behavior Yael Niv, Jeffrey A. Edlund, Peter Dayan, and John O’Doherty Cohen.
Does the brain compute confidence estimates about decisions?
It is believed that when inter-trial time is not controlled, the CI effect will occur (i.e., random practice will outperform blocked practice in retention),
Rescorla-Wagner Model  US-processing model  Can account for some Pavlovian Conditioning phenomena: acquisition blocking unblocking with an upshift conditioned.
PSY402 Theories of Learning
US location switch alone
Volume 53, Issue 1, Pages 9-16 (January 2007)
Neuroimaging of associative learning
Risk-Responsive Orbitofrontal Neurons Track Acquired Salience
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Ranulfo Romo, Adrián Hernández, Antonio Zainos  Neuron 
Shih-Chieh Lin, Miguel A.L. Nicolelis  Neuron 
Volume 15, Issue 7, Pages (April 2005)
Threat signals of pain modulate defensive responses in observers: development of an experimental paradigm Goubert, L.1*, PhD, Caes, L. 1, MSc, Uzieblo,
Volume 72, Issue 4, Pages (November 2011)
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Daphna Shohamy, Anthony D. Wagner  Neuron 
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Neuroimaging of associative learning
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Volume 95, Issue 6, Pages e3 (September 2017)
Hannah M. Bayer, Paul W. Glimcher  Neuron 
Dopamine Reward Prediction Error Responses Reflect Marginal Utility
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Volume 87, Issue 5, Pages (September 2015)
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Ju Tian, Naoshige Uchida  Neuron 
Wallis, JD Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute UC, Berkeley
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Neuroimaging of associative learning
Volume 87, Issue 5, Pages (September 2015)
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Sara E. Morrison, Alexandre Saez, Brian Lau, C. Daniel Salzman  Neuron 
Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum  John P. O'Doherty, Tony W.
Temporal Specificity of Reward Prediction Errors Signaled by Putative Dopamine Neurons in Rat VTA Depends on Ventral Striatum  Yuji K. Takahashi, Angela J.
Volume 92, Issue 2, Pages (October 2016)
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Judging Peripheral Change: Attentional and Stimulus-Driven Effects
Matthew R. Roesch, Adam R. Taylor, Geoffrey Schoenbaum  Neuron 
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Presentation transcript:

Neural Correlates of Variations in Event Processing during Learning in Basolateral Amygdala Matthew R. Roesch*, Donna J. Calu, Guillem R. Esber, and Geoffrey Schoenbaum * Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland College Park (2010)

Background… To optimize reward, animals must learn to associate cues with rewards and recognize the difference between the reward expected and that which actually occurs to guide their behaior The prediction error 2 categories of learning models:

Category 1: “Signed error” Models If a reward is larger than expected(+), the association between the cue and reward will be strengthened, whereas if the reward is smaller than expected(-), the association will be weakened. …predict that the sign of the prediction error (i.e., whether the reward is bigger or smaller than expected) will be encoded in neural activity.

Category 1 This correlate has been shown in midbrain dopamine neurons (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972; Sutton and Barto, 1998; Mirenowicz and Schultz, 1994; Montague et al., 1996; Schultz et al., 1997; Hollerman and Schultz, 1998; Waelti et al., 2001; Bayer and Glimcher, 2005; Pan et al., 2005; Bayer et al., 2007; D'Ardenne et al., 2008; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 200 9). Firing in these neurons increases in the face of unexpected reward (+) and is suppressed when reward is unexpectedly omitted (-). Evidence also from other brain areas (Hong and Hikosaka, 2008; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009).

Category 2: “Unsigned error’ models Prediction errors tell an animal that it must learn more about the cue–reward association and therefore serve to drive attention. A cue should be more thoroughly processed (and learned about) when it is a poor predictor of reward. When the cue becomes a more reliable predictor, processing (and learning) should decline (Pearce–Hall model (1980, 1982)).

Category 2 These models predict that neural activity encoding prediction errors will be similar regardless of the sign of the error(+/-). …lack of evidence for neural correlates of unsigned prediction errors—e.g., increased firing when reward is either better or worse than expected.

What did this paper do? Basolateral Amygdalar (ABL) Neurons Encode Unsigned Prediction Errors. This neural signal increased immediately after a change in reward, and stronger firing was evident whether the value of the reward increased or decreased.

How did they do it Recording single unit activity in a behavioral task in which rewards were unexpectedly delivered or omitted. Basic paradigm is a choice task Reward well Odor Port Reward well 3 different odor cues: one signaled reward on the right (forced-choice), a second for left (forced-choice), and a third for either well (free-choice).

Trials and Blocks Each Block consists of at least 60 trials; In between blocks, rewarding value shifted (i.e. value of the port for rats changed) < < > >

Results Performance and recording sites

70 reward-responsive ABL neurons recorded ; 58/70 exhibited differential firing base on timing (short/long delay) or size of the reward(large/small) after learning,  signed coding theory; outcome-selective They also exhibited changes in reward-related firing between the beginning and end trials of each block, regardless reward upshift or downshift,  unsigned coding theory

2 factor ANOVA analysis in each neuron across learning (early vs late) and shift type (upshift vs downshift) 10 of the 58 neurons (17%) fired significantly more early in a block(after a change in reward), than later(after learning).

Indices [(early – late)/(early + late)], representing the difference in firing to reward delivery (within 1 s) during trials 3–10 (early) and during the last 10 trials (late) after shifts.

Main contribution of this paper The activity in the outcome-selective ABL neurons was higher at the start of a new training block, whether reward was better or worse than expected, and declined as the rats learned to predict the value of reward. This pattern of firing is generally consistent with the notion of an “unsigned error” models such as that of Pearce and Hall (1980)

Another distinctive feature Their firing did not immediately increase at the start of a new block, in response to a change in reward, but rather appeared to gather momentum and peak a few trials into the block (3 rd trial).

Given the remarkable fit provided by the amended Pearce–Hall model (1982) and the role attributed to unsigned errors within this theoretical context, it seems natural to speculate that this ABL signal may be related to variations in event processing (  title). Especially in view of the striking similarity between changes in the ABL signal and changes in the rats' latency to approach the odor port at the start of each trial.

Increase in speed of orienting to the odor port Trial by trial analysis The close relationship between the ABL signal and a behavioral measure “speed of orienting”

Explanation Faster odor-port approach latencies may reflect error-driven increases in the processing of trial events (e.g., cues and/or reward), because rats accelerate the reception of those events when shifted contingencies need to be worked out. In this sense, approaching the odor port faster can be looked upon as similar to conditioned orienting. Conditioned orienting responses, also known as investigatory reflexes, means to recover from habituation when learned contingencies are shifted.

To further investigate the relationship between the ABL signal and odor-port approach latency, ABL was inactivated in some rats during performance of the recording task. Inactivation of ABL disrupted the change in orienting. ns

Inactivation of ABL also retarded learning in response to changes in reward. Inactivation of ABL with DNQX Block 1 well 1> well2, rats prefer well1 Block 2, well 1< well 2, rats continue to approach well 1

Choice performance in vehicle versus NBQX sessions, plotted according to whether the well values in a particular trial block were similar to or opposite from those learned at the end of the prior session.

Conclusions Basolateral Amygdalar Neurons Encode Unsigned Prediction Errors ; This neural signal was correlated with faster orienting to predictive cues after changes in reward, and abolition of it disrupted this change in orienting and retarded learning in response to changes in reward. These results suggest that basolateral amygdala serves a critical function in attention for learning.