Affective and Cognitive Consequences of Ostracism in Relation to Belongingness Motive Alp Giray Kaya, Gonca Çiffiliz, Yasemin Abayhan, Orhan Aydın, Deniz.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
We found that the humility workbook did indeed significantly increase trait humility and decreased trait negativity between pre-test and follow-up Participants.
Advertisements

The effect of pain on postoperative adjustment in patients undergoing colposcopy Susanna Kola & Jane C. Walsh 1 School of Psychology, National University.
Child Witnesses to Ostracism: Innocent Bystanders or Contributors to Social Isolation? Anne Howard, Tiffanie Almeida, John Pryor, Ph.D., & Steven Landau,
Results PASAT Mood Manipulation PANAS Outcomes. Results of the ANCOVA with PANAS as the dependent variable revealed a significant main effect for mood.
Figure 1. A Trial in the Old-Unpleasant IAT Task
They Played a Game: Reactions to Obesity Stigma in a Cyberball Game John B. Pryor & Glenn D. Reeder Illinois State University Eric D. Wesselmann, Kipling.
So is violence bad for me? Discuss the effects of short-term and long ‑ term exposure to violence.
Yasemin Abayhan, Deniz Sahin, Orhan Aydın, Gonca Ciffiliz, Hayal Yavuz, Savas Ceylan, Alp Giray Kaya Relationships Between Parental Psychological Control,
Effects of Marital Satisfaction & Personality Grace White, B.S.
The Effects of Empathy & Social Exclusion PURPOSE AND BACKGROUND Individuals’ willingness to engage in prosocial behavior is a popular topic in social.
Self-esteem Anxious attachment Avoidant attachment Model of other Perceived regard Felt Security/ insecurity ?? Partner devaluation Emotional distance.
Contact: Summary: Narcissists act aggressively toward others who give them negative, ego-threatening feedback (Bushman & Baumeister,
Discussion  The results suggest that prosocially oriented videogames have at least a short-term priming effect for prosocial thoughts, feelings, and attributed.
Social network and support influences on perceived control for exercising 2, 4 or 6 days per week S.N. Fraser 1, T.C. Murray 1,2, W.M. Rodgers 2, & C.
Ostracism refers to being ignored, excluded and/or rejected by other individuals or groups independent of offering a justification or being a target of.
Self-perception, Self-esteem and Relationship Behaviour
Between and Within Subject Measures of Affect William Revelle and Eshkol Rafaeli-Mor Northwestern University European Association of Personality Psychology.
The Need to Belong Ubuntu – my humanity is caught up, is inextricably bound up, in yours.
Biological Preparedness
2.2 A Ball of a Time! Sport Psychology.
Hayal Yavuz, Yasemin Abayhan, Savaş Ceylan, Deniz Şahin, Orhan Aydın, Alp Giray Kaya, Gonca Çiffiliz Hacettepe University, Department of Psychology Social.
Learning and Motivation. Understanding how people Learn Affective Theories.
Stress Management 2B PES. Stress Management  Arousal, anxiety and stress are all closely related.  AROUSAL: the amount of ‘readiness’ a person experiences.
Activity 3.3 Questions to Ask when Designing an Experiment In this presentation are a series of questions that you can ask yourself as you go through the.
Ecological Momentary Gratitude Intervention Produces Immediate Mood Effects Sarah Ringenberg & Emily Ragsdale Indiana Wesleyan University.
Individual Preferences for Uncertainty: An Ironically Pleasurable Stimulus Bankert, M., VanNess, K., Hord, E., Pena, S., Keith, V., Urecki, C., & Buchholz,
Interpersonal Skills 4 detailed studies Health Psychology.
Descriptive Research Study Investigation of Positive and Negative Affect of UniJos PhD Students toward their PhD Research Project Dr. K. A. Korb University.
Shocking Null-Effects in a Shock-Threat Paradigm Lisa A. Scepkowski, M.A. 1 & Erick Janssen, Ph.D. 2 1 Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders, Boston.
INTRODUCTION HYPOTHESES MEASURES RESULTS Correspondence to: at the 25 th Annual Meeting of the Association for Psychological.
INTRODUCTION HYPOTHESES MEASURES RESULTS Correspondence to: at the 27 th Association for Psychological Science Conference,
1 Understanding the Negative Impact of Racial Discrimination on HIV Risk Behaviors among African American Young Adults Michelle Stock, Ph.D. The George.
G543 Phobias. Some thinking questions: Why is it called a TV set when there's only one? Why is it called a TV set when there's only one? How can you hear.
Individual differences in statistics anxiety Donncha Hanna School of Psychology QUB.
Does Posting Status Updates Increase or Decrease Loneliness? An Online Social Networking Experiment Fenne große Deters & Matthias R. Mehl 15th General.
Promoting Connection: Perspective-taking Improves Relationship Closeness and Perceived Regard in Participants with Low Implicit Self-Esteem Julie Longua.
Recent research has shown that some rejected individuals will try to forge social connections with new individuals, which may serve to replenish a sense.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Can Comfort Food Soothe the Rejected Soul? Effects of comfort food for social and physical distress.
The Good with the Bad: Facebook as a Source of Belonging, Life Satisfaction, Stress, and Ostracism Stephanie J. Tobin, Eric J. Vanman, Marnize Verreynne,
Template provided by: “posters4research.com”   Ideals: mental constructs that represent an idea of traits we are attracted to in potential partners (Fletcher.
Stress buffering benefits of capitalization events Agnes Wolowiec, Brooklyn College, CUNY & Cheryl L. Carmichael, Brooklyn College, CUNY Method Participants.
High school still haunts us: Effects of past and current peer victimization on memory Aaliyah Gibbons, Alanna Wormwood, and Jennifer M. Knack Clarkson.
The impact of relationship social comparison interpretations on dating relationship quality over time Marian M. Morry, Tamara A. Sucharyna, Mason Legge.
Commitment Identity Motives Meaning Self Esteem Distinctiveness Continuity Belongingness Identity Motives Meaning Self Esteem Distinctiveness Continuity.
Participants 245 participants were recruited using Amazon Mechanical Turk. They were randomly assigned to the experimental group (N=117) and the control.
Participants and Procedure 1,447 participants representing 64 countries (mostly India and the United States) completed a cross-sectional survey via Amazon’s.
Introduction Inconsistent Findings in Social Rejection Literature One meta-analysis suggested social exclusion increases negative emotions (Gerber & Wheeler,
Self-Compassion and Positive Mood as Predictors of Meaning in Life: The Moderating Effect of Age Introduction “Into each life some rain must fall” (Longfellow,
Taylor Nelson and Nina S. Mounts Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University The Mediating Role of Self-Esteem on Consulting and Distress about.
Methods Participants. Participants (N = 72) were predominantly female (n = 54), Caucasian (85.5%) and (SD = 3.41) years of age. Materials and Procedure.
Accountability Pillar: Continuous Improvement – School Improvement Detail.
Anxiety Increases Adult Age Differences in Memory Julie L. Earles, Ph.D. and Alan W. Kersten, Ph.D. Class of 2005 Harriet L. Wilkes Honors College of Florida.
Ann E. Schlosser, Professor of Marketing, University of Washington Edita S. Cao, Doctoral Student of Marketing, University of Washington.
Norming Study Mechanisms of Emotion Regulation: The Role of Attentional Control Lindsey R. Wallace, M. A. & Elisabeth J. Ploran, Ph.D. Department of Psychology,
Is Social Exclusion a Moral Issue?
Better to Give or to Receive?: The Role of Dispositional Gratitude
Do Adoptees Have Lower Self Esteem?
Zachary Hohman, Elizabeth Niedbala, & Ethan Dahl
Eleni-Ilianna Mavropoulou & Kenneth E
1 Australian Catholic University; 2 The University of Queensland
Difference in Mls poured between the subject and the researcher
Stephanie J. Tobin1, Sarah McDermott2, and Luke French2
Parental Affective Reactions to Prolonged Infant Crying: Does Risk Status or Parent Gender Matter? Kreila Cote, Christie Miksys, Sapir Sasson, Jennifer.
University of Mount Union
Wrap up & Questions Melanie Kopaska.
Assessing Cognitive and Emotional Features of Real-Life Hoping
Jacob A. Jardel Cameron University Dr. Jenel T. Cavazos
Taylor Anderson Minnesota State University Moorhead
positive & negative affect schedule
The Brief Mood Introspection Scale (BMIS) 30 Years Later: Norms and Validity Evidence from a Meta-Analysis . Rachael M. Cavallaro, Victoria M. Bryan.
Presentation transcript:

Affective and Cognitive Consequences of Ostracism in Relation to Belongingness Motive Alp Giray Kaya, Gonca Çiffiliz, Yasemin Abayhan, Orhan Aydın, Deniz Şahin, Hayal Yavuz & Savaş Ceylan Hacettepe University Social Psychology Laboratory

Ostracism Ostracism is generally defined as being ignored and excluded (Williams et. al., 2005) Ostracism is generally defined as being ignored and excluded (Williams et. al., 2005) Without excessive explanation or explicit negative attention (Williams, 2007) Without excessive explanation or explicit negative attention (Williams, 2007) Williams, K.D., Forgas, J.P., von Hippel, W., & Zadro, L. (2005). The social outcast: An overview. In K.D. Williams, J.P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The Social Outcast: Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection and Bullying (pp.1-16). NY: Psychology Press. Williams, K.D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58,

Threatened Needs Ostracism threatens four needs in humans: Ostracism threatens four needs in humans: Belonging Belonging Control Control Self-esteem Self-esteem Meaningful existence Meaningful existence

Reactions to Ostracism Immediate reactions Immediate reactions Pain, hurt feelings, physiological arousal Pain, hurt feelings, physiological arousal Short-term reactions Short-term reactions Attempts to regain needs Attempts to regain needs Long-term reactions Long-term reactions Learned helplessness, low self-esteem, suicidal thoughts Learned helplessness, low self-esteem, suicidal thoughts

Moderating Factors on Reactions Attributions Attributions Responsibility, control, self-other blame Responsibility, control, self-other blame Individual differences Individual differences Attachment styles, needs for belonging, control, self-esteem Attachment styles, needs for belonging, control, self-esteem

Current Study The aim of this study was to explore, The aim of this study was to explore, Effects of being informed prior to ostracism Effects of being informed prior to ostracism Effects of the participants’ prior level of need to belong on, Effects of the participants’ prior level of need to belong on, Needs of control, self-esteem, belonging and meaningful existence Needs of control, self-esteem, belonging and meaningful existence Positive and negative mood Positive and negative mood

Participants 120 students participated the study 120 students participated the study 11 students were excluded from analysis for not completing the scales properly. 11 students were excluded from analysis for not completing the scales properly. All of the participants were freshman students in Hacettepe University or Izmir University of Economics. All of the participants were freshman students in Hacettepe University or Izmir University of Economics.

Procedure: Need to Belong Scale Need to belong scale (Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, Schreindorfer, in press) was conducted to freshman students as potential participants 2-3 weeks before the experiments began. Need to belong scale (Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, Schreindorfer, in press) was conducted to freshman students as potential participants 2-3 weeks before the experiments began. 5 point likert scale: higher scores indicate higher need 5 point likert scale: higher scores indicate higher need Alpha=.70 Alpha=.70

Procedure: Cover Story Participants were taken into the lab in 4 person groups and given consent forms including the cover story. Participants were taken into the lab in 4 person groups and given consent forms including the cover story. They were told that they would be participating an experiment on “mental visualisation skills” They were told that they would be participating an experiment on “mental visualisation skills”

Procedure: Cyberball Game Participants were told that in order to assess their mental visualisation skills, they were to play a “ball- game” over internet in four people groups. Participants were told that in order to assess their mental visualisation skills, they were to play a “ball- game” over internet in four people groups. The Cyberball game is developed by Williams, Cheung and Choi (2000), in order to manipulate ostracism condition. The Cyberball game is developed by Williams, Cheung and Choi (2000), in order to manipulate ostracism condition. The game is consisted of ball throwings between cartoon figures representing each player, lasting approximately 50 throws. The game is consisted of ball throwings between cartoon figures representing each player, lasting approximately 50 throws. In ostracism condition, after the first 2-3 throws the participant does not get the ball anymore. In ostracism condition, after the first 2-3 throws the participant does not get the ball anymore. In control condition (all-included) participant gets the ball as often as the others do. In control condition (all-included) participant gets the ball as often as the others do. Williams, K.D., Cheung, C.K.T., & Choi, W. (2000). CyberOstracism: Effects of being ignored over the internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,

Procedure: PANAS and Need Threat Scale After the completion of cyberball game, participants were asked to fill two scales After the completion of cyberball game, participants were asked to fill two scales Positive-Negative Affect Scale (Watson, Clark & Tellegen, 1988; adopted by Gençöz, 2000) - 7 point Positive-Negative Affect Scale (Watson, Clark & Tellegen, 1988; adopted by Gençöz, 2000) - 7 point Positive affect: higher scores indicate higher positive mood Positive affect: higher scores indicate higher positive mood Negative affect: higher scores indicate higher negative mood Negative affect: higher scores indicate higher negative mood Need Threat Scale (Van Beest & Williams, 2006; adopted by res. gr.) – 7 point Need Threat Scale (Van Beest & Williams, 2006; adopted by res. gr.) – 7 point Belongingness Belongingness Self-esteem Self-esteem Control Control Meaningful existence Meaningful existence Van Beest, I, & Williams, K.D. (2006). When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Gençöz, T. (2000). Pozitif ve negatif duygu ölçeği: Geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 15, Higher scores indicate lower threat

Design A 2: need to belong level (high vs. low) x 3: ostracism condition (informed ostracism, ostracism vs. control) design was used. A 2: need to belong level (high vs. low) x 3: ostracism condition (informed ostracism, ostracism vs. control) design was used. PANAS scores and need threat scale scores were analyzed by this design. PANAS scores and need threat scale scores were analyzed by this design.

Manipulation Checks Participants were asked to guess the percentage of the ball that had been thrown to them during the game. Participants were asked to guess the percentage of the ball that had been thrown to them during the game. All inclusion: % 35.5 All inclusion: % 35.5 Informed ostracism: %15.6 Informed ostracism: %15.6 Ostracism: %10.7 Ostracism: %10.7

Informed Ostracism Ostracism All Inclusion High NTB Low NTB High NTB Low NTB High NTB Low NTB PANAS Positive Affect 3.44 (1.25) 3.70 (1.21) 3.70 (1.36) 2.99 (0.82) 4.64 (1.14) 4.34 (1.02) Negative Affect 2.65 (0.95) 2.20 (1.04) 2.65 (1.17) 2.54 (1.02) 2.06 (1.31) 2.12 (1.19) Need Threat Scale Belongingness 2.84 (1.54) 3.45 (1.56) 2.48 (1.57) 2.58 (1.31) 4.87 (1.44) 5.06 (1.29) Self-Esteem 4.29 (1.61) 5.48 (1.20) 4.14 (1.38) 4.04 (1.40) 5.60 (1.20) 5.90 (1.12) Control 2.81 (1.07) 3.40 (1.54) 2.85 (1.50) 2.88 (1.46) 4.58 (1.32) 4.82 (1.18) Meaningful Existence 2.91 (1.75) 4.08 (1.54) 2.85 (1.74) 3.09 (1.52) 5.22 (1.32) 4.97 (1.47)

Results: PANAS A 2: need to belong (high vs. low) x 3: ostracism condition (informed ostracism, ostracism, all inclusion) MANOVA revealed a significant main effect of ostracism condition on PANAS scores (Wilks’  =.82, F(5, 103) = 5.29, p <.001,  ² =.09). A 2: need to belong (high vs. low) x 3: ostracism condition (informed ostracism, ostracism, all inclusion) MANOVA revealed a significant main effect of ostracism condition on PANAS scores (Wilks’  =.82, F(5, 103) = 5.29, p <.001,  ² =.09). Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on positive affect scores (F(2, 106) = 8.9, p <.001,  ² =.14). The positive affect scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions. Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on positive affect scores (F(2, 106) = 8.9, p <.001,  ² =.14). The positive affect scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions.

Results: PANAS Informed Ostracism Ostracism All Inclusion PANAS Positive Affect 3.58 (1.22) 3.41 (1.20) 4.48 (1.07)* Negative Affect 2.41 (1.01) 2.61 (1.09) 2.09 (1.23)

Results: Need Threat Scale A 2: need to belong (high vs. low) x 3: ostracism condition (informed ostracism, ostracism, all inclusion) MANOVA revealed a significant main effect of ostracism condition on Need Threat scale scores (Wilks’  =.61, F(5, 103) = 6.92, p <.001,  ² =.22). A 2: need to belong (high vs. low) x 3: ostracism condition (informed ostracism, ostracism, all inclusion) MANOVA revealed a significant main effect of ostracism condition on Need Threat scale scores (Wilks’  =.61, F(5, 103) = 6.92, p <.001,  ² =.22).

Results: Need Threat Scale Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on belongingness need scores (F(2, 106) = 26.97, p <.001,  ² =.34). The belongingness need scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions. Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on belongingness need scores (F(2, 106) = 26.97, p <.001,  ² =.34). The belongingness need scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions.

Results: Need Threat Scale Informed Ostracism Ostracism All Inclusion Need Threat Scale Belongingness 3.16 (1.56) 2.53 (1.45) 4.97 (1.35)* Self-Esteem 4.91 (1.51)* 4.10 (1.37)* 5.76 (1.15)* Control 3.12 (1.35) 2.86 (1.46) 4.71 (1.23)* Meaningful Existence 3.52 (1.72) 2.95 (1.63) 5.09 (1.39)*

Results: Need Threat Scale Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on self-esteem need scores (F(2, 106) = 12.88, p <.001,  ² =.19). The self- esteem need scores were highest in all inclusion condition, lower in informed ostracism condition and lowest in ostracism condition. All conditions differed significantly from each other. Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on self-esteem need scores (F(2, 106) = 12.88, p <.001,  ² =.19). The self- esteem need scores were highest in all inclusion condition, lower in informed ostracism condition and lowest in ostracism condition. All conditions differed significantly from each other.

Results: Need Threat Scale Informed Ostracism Ostracism All Inclusion Need Threat Scale Belongingness 3.16 (1.56) 2.53 (1.45) 4.97 (1.35)* Self-Esteem 4.91 (1.51)* 4.10 (1.37)* 5.76 (1.15)* Control 3.12 (1.35) 2.86 (1.46) 4.71 (1.23)* Meaningful Existence 3.52 (1.72) 2.95 (1.63) 5.09 (1.39)*

Results: Need Threat Scale Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on control need scores (F(2, 106) = 20.15, p <.001,  ² =.27). The control need scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions. Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on control need scores (F(2, 106) = 20.15, p <.001,  ² =.27). The control need scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions.

Results: Need Threat Scale Informed Ostracism Ostracism All Inclusion Need Threat Scale Belongingness 3.16 (1.56) 2.53 (1.45) 4.97 (1.35)* Self-Esteem 4.91 (1.51)* 4.10 (1.37)* 5.76 (1.15)* Control 3.12 (1.35) 2.86 (1.46) 4.71 (1.23)* Meaningful Existence 3.52 (1.72) 2.95 (1.63) 5.09 (1.39)*

Results: Need Threat Scale Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on meaningful existence need scores (F(2, 106) = 17.25, p <.001,  ² =.25). The meaningful existence need scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions. Follow-up ANOVA’s showed that there was a significant effect of ostracism condition on meaningful existence need scores (F(2, 106) = 17.25, p <.001,  ² =.25). The meaningful existence need scores were higher in all inclusion condition than both ostracism conditions. There was no significant difference between ostracism conditions.

Results: Need Threat Scale Informed Ostracism Ostracism All Inclusion Need Threat Scale Belongingness 3.16 (1.56) 2.53 (1.45) 4.97 (1.35)* Self-Esteem 4.91 (1.51)* 4.10 (1.37)* 5.76 (1.15)* Control 3.12 (1.35) 2.86 (1.46) 4.71 (1.23)* Meaningful Existence 3.52 (1.72) 2.95 (1.63) 5.09 (1.39)*

Discussion Results show that prior levels of need to belong had no effect on need threat scores and affect scores. Results show that prior levels of need to belong had no effect on need threat scores and affect scores. Being informed that a person is going to be ostracized by a certain rule of the game does not reduce the effects of ostracism on threatening needs and lowering affect. This result is consistent with prior studies that reactions to ostracism are automatic in nature. Being informed that a person is going to be ostracized by a certain rule of the game does not reduce the effects of ostracism on threatening needs and lowering affect. This result is consistent with prior studies that reactions to ostracism are automatic in nature.

Thank you for listening…