Neural Correlates of second- language word learning: minimal instruction produces rapid change Judith McLaughlin, Lee Osterhout & Albert Kim (2004) Presented.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Name: Date: Read temperatures on a thermometer Independent / Some adult support / A lot of adult support
Advertisements

Copyright © 2014 by McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved.
Win Big AddingSubtractEven/Odd Rounding Patterns Q $100 Q $200 Q $300 Q $400 Q $500 Q $100 Q $200 Q $300 Q $400 Q $500 Last Chance.
OPTN Modifications to Heart Allocation Policy Implemented July 12, 2006 Changed the allocation order for medically urgent (Status 1A and 1B) patients Policy.
CALENDAR.
Lecture 2 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE: AN INTRODUCTION
1 Contact details Colin Gray Room S16 (occasionally) address: Telephone: (27) 2233 Dont hesitate to get in touch.
Chapter 7 Sampling and Sampling Distributions
The 5S numbers game..
Biostatistics Unit 5 Samples Needs to be completed. 12/24/13.
The basics for simulations
EE, NCKU Tien-Hao Chang (Darby Chang)
Elementary Statistics
Early studies of the development of recognition memory in infants demonstrated a negative component over central leads (Nc) with greater amplitude in event-related.
CHAPTER 16 Life Tables.
Chapter 16 Goodness-of-Fit Tests and Contingency Tables
Chi-Square and Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)
Cost-Volume-Profit Relationships
Helen Gaeta, David Friedman, & Gregory Hunt Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory New York State Psychiatric Institute Differential Effects of Stimulus.
MCQ Chapter 07.
Accessing spoken words: the importance of word onsets
Sets Sets © 2005 Richard A. Medeiros next Patterns.
Before Between After.
Please enter data on page 477 in your calculator.
Rickleton Primary School Calculation Policy
Subtraction: Adding UP
Equal or Not. Equal or Not
Putting Statistics to Work
Statistical Inferences Based on Two Samples
© The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., Chapter 10 Testing the Difference between Means and Variances.
© The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., Chapter 12 Chi-Square.
Chapter Thirteen The One-Way Analysis of Variance.
Tutorial: Understanding the normal curve. Gauss Next mouse click.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Chapter 11: The t Test for Two Related Samples
Experimental Design and Analysis of Variance
Introduction to Management Science
Using Lowest Common Denominator to add and subtract fractions
January Structure of the book Section 1 (Ch 1 – 10) Basic concepts and techniques Section 2 (Ch 11 – 15): Inference for quantitative outcomes Section.
Evaluating the Effect of Neighborhood Size on Chinese Word Naming and Lexical Decision Meng-Feng Li 1, Jei-Tun WU 1*, Wei-Chun Lin 1 and Fu-Ling Yang 1.
ERPs to Semantic and Physical Anomalies in Cartoon Videos Jennifer Michelson 1, Courtney Brown 1, Laura Davis 1, Tatiana Sitnikova 2 & Phillip J. Holcomb.
Introduction How do people recognize objects presented in pictorial form? The ERP technique has been shown to be extremely useful in studies where the.
Introduction Can you read the following paragraph? Can we derive meaning from words even if they are distorted by intermixing words with numbers? Perea,
Adam Houston 1, Chris Westbury 1 & Morton Gernsbacher 2 1 Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Canada, 2 Department of Psychology, University.
PET Count  Word Frequency effects (coefficients) were reliably related to activation in both the striate and ITG for older adults only.  For older adults,
Semantic Priming Effects in a Bilingual Gujarati Speaker
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Neurofeedback of beta frequencies:
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Volume 26, Issue 14, Pages (July 2016)
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Lexical Influences on Auditory Streaming
Task-Guided Selection of the Dual Neural Pathways for Reading
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e2 (October 2017)
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
A. Saez, M. Rigotti, S. Ostojic, S. Fusi, C.D. Salzman  Neuron 
Learning Letters in Adulthood
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Volume 65, Issue 4, Pages (February 2010)
Experimental procedures.
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Presentation transcript:

Neural Correlates of second- language word learning: minimal instruction produces rapid change Judith McLaughlin, Lee Osterhout & Albert Kim (2004) Presented by: Qinghua Tang Oct 25, 2006

Background Adult second-language (L2) learning is often considered slow and laborious compared to how fast children acquire their first language. But, is it the case that all aspects of L2 learning are uniformly slow? What about the rate of L2 word learning, for example? Besides, the claim is often based on behavioral performances of L2 learners What about the neurophysiological status of the learners? Is there a gap between the behavioral performance of L2 learners and their neurophysiological status?

The goal of the study Decide how much L2 exposure is needed before an adult L2 learner’s brain activity reflects the lexical status(i.e. whether the letter string is a legitimate word or not) and meaning of L2 words?

Experimental design: technique ERP recording provides a nearly continuous sampling of the brain’s electrical activity. N400 reflects word status and word meaning native speakers of a language are supposed to show the largest N400 amplitude for pronounceable pseudowords, second largest for words preceded by semantically unrelated words, smallest for words preceded by semantically related words That is, for N400 amplitude pseudowords > unprimed words > primed words

Experimental design: process (1) Three sessions all together: Session 1: Mean 14 hours of instruction(range 5-28 hours) Session 2: mean 63 hours of instruction(range hours) session 3: mean 138 hours of instruction(range hours)

Experiment design: process (2) In each session: Participants made a lexical decision for each of the prime-target pair. Each trial started with fixation cross (500ms), followed by blank screen (500ms), prime (400ms), target (400ms), response prompt ERP was recorded at the same time at 200Hz from 13 scalp sites (three midline, five lateral pairs)

Subjects A group (n = 18, 16,13 for each session respectively) of university students taking a 9-month long introductory French course (mean age: 21.3 years) A control group (n = 8) who had never had significant exposure to French (mean age 27.6 years) All the participants had received at least 1 year of instruction in another foreign language 5 learners dropped the course All the participants were included in single session analyses, but only those who participated fully were included in multi-session comparisons.

stimuli 2 lists of 112 ‘prime- target’ pairs of letter strings, including: 40 semantically related pairs. E.g. chien - chat 40 semantically unrelated pairs. E.g. maison – soif 32 pairs in the pattern of word-pseudoword E.g. mot - nasier Target words were identical across lists but counterbalanced across prime type Each participant saw one list per session The list in session 1 was repeated in session 3

Results: behavioral Non-learners showed no sensitivity in lexical decision task in all three sessions Sensitivity is measured using a d’ representing the likelihood of a real target being recognized (d’=0 indicates no sensitivity, d’ = 4 indicates near perfect sensitivity.) Learners showed no sensitivity in session 1, but with moderate increase in sensitivity in session 2 and 3

Table 1 Proportion of words and pseudowords identified as a word in the lexical decision task, and the d' measure of sensitivity Words Related UnrelatedPseudowordsd'd' Nonlearners Session Session Session French learners Session Session Session We calculated d' using a formula provided by Miller 8 : d' = z (h) - z (fa), where d' represents the likelihood of a real target being recognized, z (h) represents the z-score that corresponds to the proportion of real words that were identified as words (hits), and z (fa) represents the z-score that corresponds to the proportion of nonwords that were identified as words (false alarms). From

Results: N400 No change observed in N400 amplitude for non- learners across all three sessions Learners showed larger N400 amplitude for pseudowords than for unprimed and primed words. The word-pseudoword difference started in session 1 and increased across all three sessions. By session 3, learners’ ERP responses were qualitatively similar to analogous native language responses. There was no significant difference between unrelated words and related words for learners in session 1 N400 effects were evenly distributed over midline sites and posteriorly distributed over lateral sites

Figure 1. Event-related potentials to target stimuli. (a) ERPs to word and pseudoword targets during the three testing sessions, for the nonlearners (n = 8; mean age: 27.6 years) and French learners (n = 18, 16 and 13 for sessions 1, 2 and 3, respectively; mean age, 21.3 years). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Data acquired over the central midline site (Cz) are shown. The vertical calibration bar indicates target onset. Each tick mark represents 100 ms. (b) Learners' ERPs to targets, averaged over sessions. ATL/R, anterior temporal left/right; TL/R, temporal left/right; WL/R, Wernicke's area left/right. Trial sequence: fixation cross (500 ms); blank screen (500 ms); prime (400 ms); blank screen (400 ms); target (400 ms); blank screen (400 ms); response prompt. Electroencephalographic activity was sampled at 200 Hz from 13 scalp sites (three midline, five lateral pairs; 0.01−100 Hz bandpass; 3 dB cut- off; left mastoid reference). Trials contaminated by artifacts (17%) were excluded. N400 amplitude was quantified as mean voltage within a 300−500 ms window, relative to a baseline of mean voltage from 100 ms before to 50 ms after stimulus onset. We used a repeated-measures ANOVA with the Greenhouse-Geisser correction. From

Discussion (1) Learners showed a word - pseudoword N400 effect after approximately 14 hours of instruction. Did the presence of this N400 effect result from L2 exposure? Both N400 amplitude and d’ score of session 1 were regressed onto the number of instructional hours for each learner N400 amplitude difference was correlated with hours of instruction. d’ score was not correlated with hours of instruction N400 differences in session 1 were also correlated with the proportion and frequency of target words in the text material assigned to each learner before session 1 Therefore, the N400 effect was a result of L2 exposure

Figure 2 Figure 2. Session 1 N400 amplitude word/pseudoword differences and d' scores regressed onto hours of instruction before session 1. (a) N400 amplitude difference between words and pseudowords. (b) d' scores. Two subjects were excluded from the d' analysis due to technical problems. From

Discussion (2) According to Saffran, Alison & newport (1996), people rapidly extract co-occurrence statistics for letter and sound combintaions within a language. Was the N400 difference between pseudowords and words due to the smaller mean grapheme co- occurrence values for the pseudowords than for words? In other words, did the graphemes of words co- occur more often than pseudowords and therefore caused the N400 difference?

Discussion (2) continued The digram, trigram and quadragram frequencies of all the French words from the first four chapters of the textbook were computed. The words and pseudowords did not differ in bigram and trigram frequency(P>0.3), but there was a difference in quadragram frequency (t 128 =1.76, P<0.04), however, the quadragram frequency was highly correlated with target word frequency(r=0.99). Therefore, the results of the study probably reflected whole-word rather than grapheme co- occurrence frequency.

Conclusion Adult language learners quickly grasp information such as word form and word meaning (initially word form, and then word meaning) L2 learning is not as slow as it is often thought to be Some behavioral experiments might underestimate what has been learned.

Implications of the study The method used in the study can be extended to test the influence of L1-L2 similarity, instructional methods and learners’ age on L2 learning.

Thank you!