ZPlanned comparisons revealed significant priming effects for Morphologically related (t 10 =3.0, p<0.01) and Orthographically related (t 10 =2.7, p<0.05)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Fossils provide a record of evolution.
Advertisements

Units and Standards. In science, numbers arent just numbers. They need a unit. We use standards for this unit. A standard is: a basis for comparison a.
1 Squares, Rectangles and Sticks Tandi Clausen-May Click the mouse to continue.
Study Guide. a) Any number less than 10 b) The comparison of two different things c) There are 11 teachers in the sixth grade and 240 students in the.
Do You Know Your Conversions?. Metric Conversions Click on the space bar when you want to reveal the answers. 1 cm = ____ mm 1 dm = ____ cm 1 m = _____.
MATHCOUNTS TOOLBOX Facts, Formulas and Tricks
Comparison between Actin Filament Models: Coarse-Graining Reveals Essential Differences Marissa G. Saunders, Gregory A. Voth Structure Volume 20, Issue.
Prime and Composite Numbers
1. What number does the following array represent?
What are the primes under 100?
Prime Factor Tree © Richard A. Medeiros 2008 next.
Prime Factorization.
GREATEST COMMON FACTOR
Factor trees.
EXAMPLE 1 Finding the Greatest Common Factor Find the greatest common factor of 56 and 84. SOLUTION STEP 1 Write the prime factorization of each number.
36 / \ 4 x 9 / \ / \ 2 x 2 x 3 x 3 = 2 2 x 3 2 by D. Fisher.
© T Madas =2 x 2 x 3 12 =2 x 3 Write these numbers as a product of prime factors =2 x =2 x.
© T Madas From the numbers in the list below, find: the square numbers.
$100 $200 $300 $400 $100 $200 $300 $400 $300 $200 $100 Place value up to 10,000 Place value up to 1,000,000 Comparisons up to 1,000,000 Prime / composite.
BEST Survey 2010 City report: Oslo Benchmarking in European Service of public Transport.
Appendix 6 College Comparisons. Mean Total Score by College (Possible Score Range 400 to 500) SSD = Total Scores for Colleges of Business, Education,
Selection Sort Comparison Data Movement Sorted.
Workshop Activity Comparative politics: How do we compare political systems?
Starter Questions 42 o. Visual Times Tables Click on each circles to view the times tables 2x 3x 4x 5x 6x 7x 8x 9x 10x.
Comparison of Muscle Types
Prime Numbers and Factoring. Which expression is equal to 4x6? (a) 2 × 2 x 2 (b) 2 × 3 × 4 (c) 4 × 5 × 3 (d) 6 × 6.
160 as a product of its prime factors is 2 5 x 5 Use this information to show that 160 has 12 factors.
Thinking about thinking. Buying a car! What questions might you ask yourself when trying to decide which is best for you?
Prime Factorization. What is Prime Factorization? The process of converting a number into a product of prime numbers.
Prime Factor Form 18-Mar-16 More free resources available from: free-online-calculator.netfree-online-calculator.net.
Buckingham School
Q: How do you know a God who is completely ‘other’? A: You need a mediator.
Which of these is 52 written as a product of its prime factors? a) 2 x 26b) 2 x 2 x 13 c) 4 x 13d) 1 x 52.
EVEN NUMBERS EVEN NUMBERS 1 = prime 2 = prime1 3 = prime 4 = 2 x 22 5 = prime 6 = 2 x 33 7 = prime 8 = 2 x 2 x 24 9 = 3 x 3 10 = 2 x 55.
Introduction to Factoring
Literacy Research Memory Skill Practice Stretch!
Starter Questions 42o.
Iron deficiency anemia
PRIME AND COMPOSITE NUMBERS
Location, Location, Location: Microglia Are Where They Live
Morphological Image Processing
Click on a numbered square to make the square disappear.
Do you know population SD? Use Z Test Are there only 2 groups to
Prime Factorization Practice
Comparison of the P-value significance between Wild Type and ΔGLN3
Prime and Composite Numbers
RGFP-966 increases H4K12 acetylation in HEK/APPsw cells.

Prime numbers.
Comparison of the P-value significance between Wild Type and dGLN3
Prime and Composite Numbers
Literacy Research Memory Skill Practice Stretch!
Why are they significant?
CCTV Inspection Results
Characterization of the Dopamine Defect in Primary Cultures of Dopaminergic Neurons from Hypoxanthine Phosphoribosyltransferase Knockout Mice  Doug W.
Word Word Word Word Word Picture Definition Historical Significance
Starter Questions 42o.
Starter Questions 42o.
Starter Questions 42o.
Prime Numbers.
Elementary Mathematics
Prime and Composite Numbers
Prime and Composite.
Prime Factorization Using Factor Trees
Fig. 5. Morphological changes of the N
Fig. 3. BCL11A acts as a silencer of γ-globin gene expression, based on modulation of BCL11A levels. BCL11A acts as a silencer of γ-globin gene expression,
(A) Mean maximum single-jaw forces (± s.d.), by species.
Knockdown of ZBP1 in hippocampal neurons decreases dendritic branching
Greatest Common Factor GCF
Comparison of pre- and post-bronchodilator data for spirometric and oscillometric parameters. Comparison of pre- and post-bronchodilator data for spirometric.
Presentation transcript:

zPlanned comparisons revealed significant priming effects for Morphologically related (t 10 =3.0, p<0.01) and Orthographically related (t 10 =2.7, p<0.05) word pairs relative to unrelated pairs. zThere was a significant BOLD signal reduction for semantically related pairs in a region of the left middle temporal gyrus (–70, –42, 4; Z=3.3), which also showed a smaller effect for morphologically related pairs (Z=2.6; Figure 7). Figure 7: Neural priming effects in the left middle temporal gyrus. zThere was a significant main effect of Meaning (F 1,21 =4.4, p<0.05) and a significant interaction (F 1,21 =8.3, p<0.01) but no effect of Form (F 1,21 =1.1). zDespite a comparable mean reduction in BOLD signal, no effect was seen for orthographic relatedness due to greater variance between subjects. Thus, both semantically and morphologically related word pairs significantly reduced BOLD signal in this region. zMorphological priming led to a reduced BOLD signal in the posterior angular gyrus, bilaterally (Figure 5) Figure 5: Neural priming effects in the posterior angular gyrus. The effects are color-coded to indicate the condition which led to the reduction. White indicates a significant priming effect in all three conditions. The adjacent bar plots illustrate the mean percent BOLD signal change in the region between each priming condition and the unrelated baseline. zThere was: 1. a significant main effect of Meaning (F 1,21 =5.7, p<0.05), 2. a marginal effect of Form (F 1,21 =3.1, p<0.1), and 3. a significant interaction (F 1,21 =12.8, p<0.01). The morphological priming effect was significantly smaller than expected in light of the main effects of Form and Meaning (i.e. a sub-additive interaction). zThe initial analysis of the imaging data identified the neural circuit engaged in word reading by contrasting Unrelated pairs to Consonant letter strings Figure 4: Regions activated by Unrelated > Consonants shown on the group mean structural scan in standard space. Scanned using gradient echo EPI, B 0 =3T, TR = 3sec, TE = 30msec, FOV = 192 × 256mm, matrix = 64 × 64. Images were realigned to correct for head motion, registered to the MNI- 152 template, and smoothed with a 5mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. RFX are shown with voxel thresholding at Z > zThis revealed activation in several areas including the: 1) left frontal operculum, 2) left precentral gyrus, 3) posterior angular gyrus, bilaterally, 4) left posterior occipito-temporal cortex, and 5) left middle temporal gyrus (not shown). consistent with previous lexical decision experiments (1-3). zWithin this system, we looked for reductions in BOLD signal for the morphological priming condition relative to the unrelated condition. These were identified in the angular gyrus, posterior occipito-temporal cortex, and middle temporal gyrus (Figures 5-7). zNo increases in BOLD signal were observed for any priming conditions. zTwo pre-tests were conducted before scanning to verify the efficacy of our visual masking zPerformance was at ceiling for words presented for 200msec. In contrast, words presented for 33msec were nearly impossible to perceive. Only 4/220 words were read aloud and performance on the matching task was not different from chance (binomial test, p=0.98). zThese results demonstrate the efficacy of the visual masking used here. Is morphology a fundamental component of language? J. T. Devlin 1, H. L. Jamison 1, P. M. Matthews 1, L. M. Gonnerman 2 1 Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain, University of Oxford, U. K. 2 Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, PA, U. S. A. Morphology is the aspect of language concerned with the internal structure of words, but it is not clear whether morphology is a basic element of linguistic structure or whether it emerges from systematic regularities between the form and meaning of words. Here we looked for evidence of morphological structure at a neural systems level using a visual masked priming paradigm and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Form and meaning relations were manipulated in a 2 2 design to identify reductions in BOLD signal related to shared form (corner-corn), to shared meaning (idea-notion), and to shared morphemes (boldly-bold, which overlapped in both form and meaning). Relative to unrelated pairs (ozone-hero), morphologically related items reduced activation in posterior angular gyrus bilaterally, left occipito- temporal cortex, and left middle temporal gyrus. In the posterior angular gyrus region, a reduction in activation was observed for all three priming conditions, possibly reflecting reduced attentional demands rather than overlapping linguistic representations per se. In contrast, the reductions seen in the left occipito-temporal cortex and left middle temporal gyrus corresponded, respectively, to main effects of orthographic and semantic overlap. In other words, the most significant finding of our experiment is that the neural regions sensitive to morphological structure appear to overlap almost entirely with regions sensitive to orthographic and semantic relatedness, suggesting that morphology emerges from the convergence of form and meaning. z11 native British English speakers participated in a visual masked priming experiment using lexical decision (Figure 1) Figure 1: A schematic diagram of the visual masked priming paradigm. The prime was forward masked by a visual noise pattern and backward masked by the target. Lexical stimuli were matched for familiarity, frequency, imageability and syllable length, while all stimuli were matched for letter length. zThe experiment used a 2 × 2 factorial design where prime-target pairs were related in Form [± Orth] and/or Meaning [± Sem] (Table) Prediction: If morphology is a separate form of linguistic information, then we would expect to see evidence of its contribution to word recognition above and beyond the effects of form and meaning (i.e. a significant supra-additive interaction). On the other hand, if morphology arises from the convergence of form and meaning, then we would expect brain regions sensitive to morphological structure to overlap with those showing sensitivity to orthographic and/or semantic structure. Current Study Behavioural Results zLike previous studies (4), we found significant behavioural priming effects for morphologically related word pairs, indicating that participants could benefit from the prime-target relation despite not being consciously aware of the prime. zResults demonstrate several neural regions sensitive to morphological relations between prime and target word pairs 1. Posterior angular gyrus: We observed reduced BOLD signal for all three priming conditions but no effect of morphology beyond that of form and meaning. In addition, in all three priming conditions response latencies decreased relative to the unrelated condition, suggesting the common reduction in activation may reflect reduced attentional demands, rather than overlapping linguistic representations per se (5-7). 2. Left occipito-temporal cortex: There was reduced BOLD signal for word pairs sharing Form (i.e. Orthographic and Morphologically related conditions), suggesting the effect may be due to orthographic overlap. This is consistent with many studies implicating this region in visual form processing (8-11). 3. Left middle temporal gyrus: We found significantly reduced activation for semantically related word pairs (i.e. Semantic and Morphologically related conditions). This is a multi-modal association area sensitive to both auditory and visual input (12,13) and is engaged in semantic processing regardless of the input (14). zThe most significant finding of our experiment is that the neural regions sensitive to morphological relatedness appear to overlap almost entirely with regions sensitive to orthographic and semantic relatedness. Power analyses demonstrated that this experiment had a sensitivity of 0.8 or greater for detecting neural priming effects, based on the effect sizes (ranging from % signal change) and standard deviations (ranging from %) observed in the current experiment. Thus, while one cannot rule out the possibility that regions not detected in this study also may contribute to morphological processing, the current findings are strikingly consistent with the claim that morphology emerges from the convergence of form and meaning. #MO16 References 1000ms 500ms 33ms 200ms Time 1500ms * passive PASS * $%##$% cast #$%#$ 1000ms 500ms 33 or 200ms 500ms Time * $%##$% wave #$%#$ WAVE MUST Task:1. Read the word aloud, 2. Select the word between the if possible. masks. If uncertain, guess. Figure 2:Behavioural pre-tests. On the left is a schematic of the reading task while the right shows the forced choice masking task. In both, words were presented for either 33 or 200msec between visual masks. Summary Imaging Results: Lexical Decision L X = -40Z = 26 Morphological Priming: Posterior Angular Gyrus LR Morphological Priming: Left middle temporal gyrus Discussion Table: Conditions and sample stimuli in the lexical decision task ConditionExample Relation 1. Unrelatedevent – RUG[–Orth, –Sem] 2. Orthographic winsome –WIN[+Orth, –Sem] 3. Semantic profit – GAIN[–Orth, +Sem] 4. Morphological hunter – HUNT[+Orth, +Sem] 5. Pseudo-words dollar – TAVENone 6. Consonant stringsbrutally – PLBNone zThere was a significant BOLD signal reduction for orthographically related pairs in a region of the left posterior occipito-temporal cortex (–44, –60, –18, Z=3.8), which also showed a smaller effect for morphologically related pairs (Z=2.7; Figure 6). Figure 6: Neural priming effects in the left occipito-temporal junction. The adjacent bar plots illustrate the mean percent BOLD signal change in the region between the priming conditions and the unrelated baseline. zThere was a significant main effect of Form (F 1,21 =15.2, p<0.001) but no effect of Meaning (F 1,21 <1), and a marginal interaction (F 1,21 =3.8, p<0.1) Both orthographically and morphologically related word pairs reduced BOLD signal in this region, although the reduction was smaller for morphologically related words. Morphological Priming: Left occipito-temporal cortex 1.Binder, J. R. et al. (2003). Neural correlates of lexical access during visual word recognition. J Cogn Neurosci, 15(3), Price, C. J., Wise, R. J., Watson, J. D., Patterson, K., Howard, D., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1994). Brain activity during reading: The effects of exposure duration and task. Brain, 117(6), Rumsey, J. M., Horwitz, B., Donahue, B. C., Nace, K., Maisog, J. M., & Andreason, P. (1997). Phonological and orthographic components of word recognition: a PET- rCBF study. Brain, 120(5), Taft, M., & Forster, K. (1975). Lexical storage and retriveal of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, Constantinidis, C., & Steinmetz, M. A. (2001). Neuronal responses in area 7a to multiple stimulus displays: II. responses are suppressed at the cued location. Cereb Cortex, 11(7), Mesulam, M.-M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, Cohen, L., Lehericy, S., Chochon, F., Lemer, C., Rivard, S., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the visual word form area. Brain, 125, Dehaene, S. et al. (2001). Cerebral mechannisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4, Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2003). The myth of the visual word form area. Neuroimage, 19(3), Vuilleumier, P., Henson, R. N., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event related fMRI of repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 5(5), Cardillo, E., Utman, J. A., Matthews, P. M., & Devlin, J. T. (in press). Left inferior prefrontal cortex activity reflects inhibitory, rather than facilitatory, priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 13.Devlin, J. T., Matthews, P. M., & Rushworth, M. F. (2003). Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci, 15(1), Vandenberghe, R., Price, C. J., Wise, R., Josephs, O., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1996). Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature, 383, Figure 3: Behavioural results of the main lexical decision task. Lexical conditions are in grey and non-lexical in black. zThere was a significant main effect of Form (F 1,10 =8.2, p<0.05) indicating that when prime-target pairs shared visual form, this facilitated responses. No other effects were significant. **