Evaluation of Dummy-Head HRTFs in the Horizontal Plane based on the Peak-Valley Structure in One-degree Spatial Resolution Wersényi György SZÉCHENYI ISTVÁN.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Frequency response adaptation in binaural hearing David Griesinger Cambridge MA USA
Advertisements

Frequency response adaptation in binaural hearing
Morset Sound Development Sound System Calibration Example - Performing Monitor Calibration in Studios.
Sound Localization Superior Olivary Complex. Localization: Limits of Performance Absolute localization: localization of sound without a reference. Humans:
Descriptive Measures MARE 250 Dr. Jason Turner.
1 3D sound reproduction with “OPSODIS” and its commercial applications Takashi Takeuchi, PhD Chief Technical Officer OPSODIS Limited Institute of Sound.
Binaural Hearing Or now hear this! Upcoming Talk: Isabelle Peretz Musical & Non-musical Brains Nov. 12 noon + Lunch Rm 2068B South Building.
Spatial Perception of Audio J. D. (jj) Johnston Neural Audio Corporation.
Learning the cues associated with non-individualised HRTFs John Worley and Jonas Braasch Binaural and Spatial Hearing Group.
Listening Tests and Evaluation of Simulated Sound Fields Using VibeStudio Designer Wersényi György Hesham Fouad SZÉCHENYI ISTVÁN UNIVERSITY, Hungary VRSonic,
3-D Sound and Spatial Audio MUS_TECH 348. Wightman & Kistler (1989) Headphone simulation of free-field listening I. Stimulus synthesis II. Psychophysical.
Localizing Sounds. When we perceive a sound, we often simultaneously perceive the location of that sound. Even new born infants orient their eyes toward.
1 Auditory Sensitivity, Masking and Binaural Hearing.
What Virtual Audio Synthesis Could Do for Visually Disabled Humans in the New Era? György Wersényi Széchenyi István University Department of Telecommunications.
All you have is a pair of instruments (basilar membranes) that measure air pressure fluctuations over time Localization.
ICA Madrid 9/7/ Simulating distance cues in virtual reverberant environments Norbert Kopčo 1, Scott Santarelli, Virginia Best, and Barbara Shinn-Cunningham.
AUDITORY PERCEPTION Pitch Perception Localization Auditory Scene Analysis.
Spectral centroid 6 harmonics: f0 = 100Hz E.g. 1: Amplitudes: 6; 5.75; 4; 3.2; 2; 1 [(100*6)+(200*5.75)+(300*4)+(400*3.2)+(500*2 )+(600*1)] / = 265.6Hz.
There are several clues you could use: 1.arrival time 2.phase lag (waves are out of sync) 3.sound shadow (intensity difference)- sound is louder at ear.
Binaural Sound Localization and Filtering By: Dan Hauer Advisor: Dr. Brian D. Huggins 6 December 2005.
1 Recent development in hearing aid technology Lena L N Wong Division of Speech & Hearing Sciences University of Hong Kong.
Frequency Modulation in Wireless Microphone System EECE 252 Project Spring 2012 Presented by: Haolin Wang, Muhamad Fuad Harun, Baihaqis Bahran.
Properties and Detection of Sound
Acoustics Reverberation.
Improved 3D Sound Delivered to Headphones Using Wavelets By Ozlem KALINLI EE-Systems University of Southern California December 4, 2003.
Applied Psychoacoustics Lecture: Binaural Hearing Jonas Braasch Jens Blauert.
Rumsey Chapter 16 Day 3. Overview  Stereo = 2.0 (two discreet channels)  THREE-DIMENSIONAL, even though only two channels  Stereo listening is affected.
3-D Sound and Spatial Audio MUS_TECH 348. Main Types of Errors Front-back reversals Angle error Some Experimental Results Most front-back errors are front-to-back.
Chapter 5: Normal Hearing. Objectives (1) Define threshold and minimum auditory sensitivity The normal hearing range for humans Define minimum audible.
Sound Vibration and Motion.
Chapter 7: Loudness and Pitch. Loudness (1) Auditory Sensitivity: Minimum audible pressure (MAP) and Minimum audible field (MAF) Equal loudness contours.
Developing a model to explain and stimulate the perception of sounds in three dimensions David Kraljevich and Chris Dove.
Modal Analysis of Rigid Microphone Arrays using Boundary Elements Fabio Kaiser.
‘Missing Data’ speech recognition in reverberant conditions using binaural interaction Sue Harding, Jon Barker and Guy J. Brown Speech and Hearing Research.
Simulation of small head-movements on a Virtual Audio Display using headphone playback and HRTF synthesis Wersényi György SZÉCHENYI ISTVÁN UNIVERSITY,
3-D Sound and Spatial Audio MUS_TECH 348. Physical Modeling Problem: Can we model the physical acoustics of the directional hearing system and thereby.
L INKWITZ L AB S e n s i b l e R e p r o d u c t i o n & R e c o r d i n g o f A u d i t o r y S c e n e s Hearing Spatial Detail in Stereo Recordings.
Figures for Chapter 14 Binaural and bilateral issues Dillon (2001) Hearing Aids.
Spatial and Spectral Properties of the Dummy-Head During Measurements in the Head-Shadow Area based on HRTF Evaluation Wersényi György SZÉCHENYI ISTVÁN.
Hearing Research Center
This research was supported by Delphi Automotive Systems
Loudness level (phon) An equal-loudness contour is a measure of sound pressure (dB SPL), over the frequency spectrum, for which a listener perceives a.
3-D Sound and Spatial Audio MUS_TECH 348. Stereo Loudspeaker Reproduction.
Listeners weighting of cues for lateral angle: The duplex theory of sound localization revisited E. A. MacPherson & J. C. Middlebrooks (2002) HST. 723.
UNIT EIGHT: Waves  Chapter 24 Waves and Sound  Chapter 25 Light and Optics.
On the improvement of virtual localization in vertical directions using HRTF synthesis and additional filtering Wersényi György SZÉCHENYI ISTVÁN UNIVERSITY,
Microphones. How Microphones Work Sound is created when a vibrating object (such as a guitar string, drum skin etc..) causes the air around it to vibrate.
3-D Sound and Spatial Audio MUS_TECH 348. Are IID and ITD sufficient for localization? No, consider the “Cone of Confusion”
Traffic noises and its abatement Baár-Madas Secondary School Budapest, Hungary.
On the manifolds of spatial hearing
PSYC Auditory Science Spatial Hearing Chris Plack.
Fletcher’s band-widening experiment (1940)
The role of reverberation in release from masking due to spatial separation of sources for speech identification Gerald Kidd, Jr. et al. Acta Acustica.
What can we expect of cochlear implants for listening to speech in noisy environments? Andrew Faulkner: UCL Speech Hearing and Phonetic Sciences.
SPATIAL HEARING Ability to locate the direction of a sound. Ability to locate the direction of a sound. Localization: In free field Localization: In free.
Numericals.
3-D Sound and Spatial Audio MUS_TECH 348. What do these terms mean? Both terms are very general. “3-D sound” usually implies the perception of point sources.
Sound Localization and Binaural Hearing
From: Acoustically Coupled Microphone Arrays
Auditory Localization in Rooms: Acoustic Analysis and Behavior
PSYCHOACOUSTICS A branch of psychophysics
What is stereophony? Stereos = solid (having dimensions: length width, height) Phonics = study of sound stereophony (stereo) is an aural illusion – a.
3D sound reproduction with “OPSODIS” and its commercial applications
C-15 Sound Physics 1.
Hearing Spatial Detail
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Localizing Sounds.
Plotting Points Guided Notes
Auditory perception: The near and far of sound localization
Presentation transcript:

Evaluation of Dummy-Head HRTFs in the Horizontal Plane based on the Peak-Valley Structure in One-degree Spatial Resolution Wersényi György SZÉCHENYI ISTVÁN UNIVERSITY, Department of Telecommunications H-9026, Győr, Egyetem tér 1., Hungary Tel.: Fax: Abstract Dummy-heads are often used for standardized measurements where modeling of the human head and torso is relevant. Monaural Head-Related Transfer Functions of a Brüel & Kjaer dummy were measured in the horizontal plane in one-degree spatial resolution. Evaluation is made by plotting the peak-valley structure in frequency. Special frequency and spatial domains can be determined based on the variations of the HRTFs that are relevant to understand physical properties of the dummy-head in measurements and processes of human directional hearing. Symmetries and similarities of measured HRTFs help to scrutinize the perception of directional information in the monaural and binaural evaluation, the “noisy domain” in frequency and space where shadowing of the head occur as well as pinna-filtering. Introduction HRTF: Head-Related Transfer Function, transmission from free-field to the eardrum(s) Where P 1 is sound pressure at the eardrum and P 2 is the sound pressure in the middle of the head recorded with omnidirectional microphone Basic localization cue for directional hearing Measurement on real humans or on dummy-heads Reproduction through (equalized) headphones In listening tests are dummy-head HRTFs inferior to individual HRTFs Dummy-heads only partly model the human body and geometry Measurement HRTFs of a Brüel Kjaer 4128 dummy-head Anechoic room, white noise excitation Resolution: 1 degree horizontal, 5 degrees vertical Accuracy in the HRTFs: 1 dB Goal: –Investigating the peak-valley structure of the bare dummy-head’s HRTFs in the horizontal plane –Investigating the role of hair, cap, glasses, clothing etc.? The peak-valley structure in the horizontal plane Typical changes in the peak-valley structure as the sound source is moving in the horizontal plane Changes of about 1 dB will be evaluated Plotting of the nearby HRTFs in one degree resolution If the figure is „thin”: nearby HRTFs are very similar 0-30º Constant increase of the overall signal level up to 3-5 dB The 9 kHz peak increases with 7-9 dB 30-80º the signal level is constant The plotted HRTFs are very similar (though this is the most sensitive spatial region) Edges are very thin, only dips and peaks are changing º: 3 kHz peak and 4 kHz valley is falling down by 4 dB and 9 dB Thin edges and significant changes at peaks and valleys (5-7 dB) º: overall signal level decreases, especially the 4 kHz valley (20 dB) Local increase of signal level is in the „back direction” º: head-shadow area Minimum of the sensitivity: º (contralateral ear) Symmetry: ±20º to the median plane Summary Evaluation of measured dummy-head transfer functions (HRTFs) was made in the horizontal plane in one degree resolution. Spectral properties were investigated by plotting nearby HRTFs in the same figure. Different spatial regions can be determined based on these figures and based on properties such as thickness, deviations and variations of the peaks and valleys in the HRTFs. We have found the minimum of the monaural sensitivity of the hearing system on the contralateral side of the head, in the head-shadow area about 255 degrees. Furthermore, symmetries to the median plane both in frontal and in back directions are clearly visible in a ±20 degrees region. Due to the symmetry of the dummy-head, the same observation and conclusion can be drawn for the other ear as well and in real (binaural) listening situation these two regions overlap. If the ear is in the head-shadow area, only some low-frequency information can be evaluated. In general we can say that the edges of nearby HRTFs do not vary significantly. This indicates that changing of the azimuth does not really influence the peak-valley structure in the frequency (no shifting) only the height of the peaks and valleys. Fig.1. The head-related coordinate system Fig.2. Horizontal plane HRTFs from 0º and 30º. Fig.3. Ten horizontal plane HRTFs from 40º to 50º. Fig.4. Horizontal plane HRTFs from 70º and 110º. Fig.5. Horizontal plane HRTFs from 90º and 140º. Fig.7. Ten plotted horizontal plane HRTFs from 350º to 359º as well as from 0º to 9º for comaprison. Note the symmetry. Fig.6. Ten plotted horizontal plane HRTFs from the head-shadow area 250º to 259º (minimum of the sensitivity). Above 1600 Hz high frequency information is variable, below there is now significant difference.