Judging Peripheral Change: Attentional and Stimulus-Driven Effects

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Left Visual Field Advantage in Asynchronous Dual-Stream RSVP Tasks: An Investigation of Potential Neural Mechanisms Andrew Clement & Nestor Matthews.
Advertisements

Attention-Dependent Hemifield Asymmetries When Judging Numerosity Nestor Matthews & Sarah Theobald Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville.
Attentionally Dependent Bilateral Advantage on Numerosity Judgments Jenny Ewing & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville.
Bilateral and Unilateral Orientation Dynamics Nestor Matthews & Kristin M. Reardon Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH USA.
Bilateral Attentional Advantage in Gabor Detection Nestor Matthews & Jenna Kelly Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH USA In.
Visual Hemifields and Perceptual Grouping Sarah Theobald & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH USA The human.
Visual attention reveals changing color in moving objects James E. Hoffman and Scott McLean University of Delaware.
Attention Orienting System and Associated Disorders Neglect, Extinction and Balint’s Syndrome.
Attention Orienting System and Associated Disorders Neglect, Extinction and Balint’s Syndrome.
I. Face Perception II. Visual Imagery. Is Face Recognition Special? Arguments have been made for both functional and neuroanatomical specialization for.
Visual Cognition II Object Perception. Theories of Object Recognition Template matching models Feature matching Models Recognition-by-components Configural.
The ‘when’ pathway of the right parietal lobe L. Battelli A. Pascual - LeoneP. Cavanagh.
The Time Course of the Oblique Effect in Orientation Sensitivity Nestor Matthews, Jennifer Cox & Alana Rojewski Department of Psychology, Denison University,
Hastening Visual Attention with Practice: A Perceptual Learning Study Michael Vawter & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University DiscussionIntroduction.
The Role of Speed Lines in Subtle Motion Judgments Jason Allen & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH USA Purpose:
Previous research has shown performance asymmetries between right and left hemifields on attention-based tasks with a disadvantage for right visual field.
Right Hemifield Deficits in Judging Simultaneity: A Perceptual Learning Study Nestor Matthews 1, Michael Vawter 1, Jenna Kelly 2 Psychology, Denison University.
Trading Noise for Stimulus Duration in Orientation Judgments Kei Kurosawa, Kristen Strong & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University,
R Driver, J. (1998). The Neuropsychology of Spatial Attention. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp ). San Diego: Psychology Press. Reviewer: Jooyoung.
# Attentional Volleying Across Visual Quadrants Andrew S. Clement 1,2 & Nestor Matthews 1 1 Department of Psychology, Denison University, 2 Department.
Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the.
In principle, the neural resources that govern attention to the left visual field (LVF) could be independent of those governing attention to the right.
An Eyetracking Analysis of the Effect of Prior Comparison on Analogical Mapping Catherine A. Clement, Eastern Kentucky University Carrie Harris, Tara Weatherholt,
Visual Performance Fields and Motor Control Robert Faludi, Laurence Maloney & Marisa Carrasco - New York University 2AFC detection task 3 observers Target:
Bilateral Superiority in Detecting Gabor Targets Among Gabor Distracters Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
Goals for Today’s Class
Effect of laterality-specific training on visual learning Jenna Kelly & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
Laterality-Specific Perceptual Learning on Gabor Detection Nestor Matthews & Jenna Kelly Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
Early Time Course Hemisphere Differences in Phonological & Orthographic Processes Laura K. Halderman 1, Christine Chiarello 1 & Natalie Kacinik 2 1 University.
Without Words for Emotions: Is the emotional processing deficit in alexithymia caused by dissociation or suppression? Christian Sinnott & Dr. Mei-Ching.
Example trial sequences for visual perspective-taking task
Feature Binding: Not Quite So Pre-attentive Erin Buchanan and M
From: Attentive and pre-attentive aspects of figural processing
Kimron Shapiro & Frances Garrad-Cole The University of Wales, Bangor
Kim Uittenhove, Lina Chaabi, Valérie Camos, Pierre Barrouillet
1 University of Hamburg 2 University of Applied Sciences Heidelberg
Jenna Kelly1,2 & Nestor Matthews2
Colour Discrimination Task
Mental Rotation of Naturalistic Human Faces
Alison Burros, Nathan Herdener, & Mei-Ching Lien
Survivor Interaction Contrast4,5
The Effects of Musical Mood and Musical Arousal on Visual Attention
The involvement of visual and verbal representations in a quantitative and a qualitative visual change detection task. Laura Jenkins, and Dr Colin Hamilton.
Nestor Matthews1, Kristin M. Reardon1&2, & Obiageli Uguru1
Unilateral Neglect, Spatial Attention, Object-Based Attention
Neural Correlates of Shape from Shading
Experimental Psychology PSY 433
Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
Double Dissociation in Radial & Rotational Motion-Defined Temporal Order Judgments Poster # Leslie Welch1, Nestor Matthews2, Elena Festa1, Kendra.
Crowding by a single bar
A Comparison of Radial and Rotational Plaid Speed Judgments
Investigating the Attentional Blink With Predicted Targets
Volume 26, Issue 4, Pages (February 2016)
Hastening Orientation Sensitivity
Perceptual Learning on Simultaneity and Temporal Order Judgments
John-Dylan Haynes, Jon Driver, Geraint Rees  Neuron 
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Perception.
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Same - Different Analysis
Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
A Comparison of Cues for Auditory Motion Judgments
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Feature-based attention
Multisensory Integration: Maintaining the Perception of Synchrony
Top-Down Modulation of Lateral Interactions in Early Vision
Perception.
Presentation transcript:

Judging Peripheral Change: Attentional and Stimulus-Driven Effects Poster # 26.314 Abstract # 413 Jenna Kelly & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH 43023 USA Introduction Method Discussion Experiment 1: Temporal Asynchrony Alone Experiment 2: Spatial vs. Temporal Previous research has revealed performance advantages for stimuli presented across (bilateral) rather than within (unilateral) the left and right hemifields on a variety of spatial attention tasks, including alphanumeric symbol identification (Awh & Pashler, 2000), conjunction and shape identification (Kraft et al., 2005), multiple object tracking (Alvarez & Cavanagh, 2005), Gabor detection and orientation discrimination (Reardon et al., 2009), and visual crowding (Chakravarthi & Cavanagh, 2009). Here, we probed for a similar advantage in a temporal attention task. Whether temporal tasks should exhibit the same bilateral advantage is non-obvious because temporal and spatial attention have different properties (Aghdaee & Cavanagh , 2007). Some of these hemifield effects emerge only in the presence of distracters. It may be important whether the distracters add difficulty in a task-relevant way; task-irrelevant difficulty may not compete for attentional resources enough to generate a bilateral advantage. Evidence from Taya et al. (2009) and Reardon et al. (2009) supports the importance of task-relevance in driving the observed hemifield effects. Here, we look for hemifield effects in a temporal attention task alone and in comparison to a spatial attention task, with a particular interest in the distinction between the properties of temporal and spatial attention and the impact of task-relevant difficulty. When distracters were absent, proficiency (d’/RT) was significantly lower in the diagonal condition than in either of the bilateral or unilateral conditions, which were statistically indistinguishable. This oblique effect was eliminated in the presence of either distracter type but maintained with the addition of the concurrent spatial task. The lower proficiency in the diagonal condition—in which targets were in opposite hemifields—prevents the attribution of this effect to laterality. This pattern differs from that observed in previous spatial attention tasks, suggesting that the properties of spatial and temporal attention may differ. We seem to have found an oblique effect, like that of Westheimer (2003), for a task in which orientation is determined by the entire configuration rather than by any of the components, and we have extended this sort of oblique effect to a timing comparison. It is possible that the effect observed here is actually one of distance; while targets are the same distance from fixation across laterality conditions, the distance across which the comparison is made is √(2) times longer on diagonal trials than on bilateral or unilateral trials. This deserves further consideration but may be tentatively refuted by the results of Kraft et al. (2005), which suggest that target-target distance may affect performance within but not between hemifields. Our apparent oblique effect might also result from the collinear relationship between the two targets and fixation on diagonal trials, which is not present on either bilateral or unilateral trials. This cannot be refuted by our data but would predict a counterintuitive effect of shifting the fixation point to a location horizontally or vertically intermediate to a pair of targets: if collinearity produces a performance deficit, then pairs of targets closer to fixation will be associated with lower performance than more distant targets that are not collinear with fixation. Such a finding would be surprising. Regardless of the reason for the lower performance on diagonal trials in this task, it is important to note that the effect generalizes across spatial and temporal attention tasks, which suggests that the two types of attention may share some properties. Even though the distracters and multitasking each made the task more difficult, the cardinal advantage was eliminated only by distracters. Thus, it is not task difficulty per se that drives the effect. It may be that the distracters, but not the concurrent spatial frequency judgment task, provides task-relevant difficulty that generates the observed oblique effect. The importance of task-relevant difficulty has also been shown for spatial attention-dependent tasks (e.g. Kraft et al., 2005). Attentional Cue Which Letter? Letter Response Prompt Peripheral Target Timing? same = s different = d Stimuli m Attentional Cue Which Letter? Letter Response Prompt Target Stripe Size? same = s different = d Peripheral Response Prompts Target Timing? different = d Stimuli m Experimental Details Training Paradigm Discriminanda: high (89.76% Michelson) contrast,14.55 deg diagonally from fixation, max/min luminances = 108.00, 5.83 cd/m2 Participants: 23 Denison University undergraduates IVs: 3 (Laterality) x 3 (Distracter) Laterality = Bilateral , Unilateral, Diagonal Distracter = Absent, Static, Dynamic Participants: 19 Denison University undergraduates IVs: 3 (Laterality) x 2 (Task) x 2 (Day) Task = Spatial vs. Temporal Day=2 (task blocked) vs. 3 (task unknown until prompt) * Results References Experiment 1: Temporal Asynchrony Alone Experiment 2: Comparing Tasks and Days Experiment 2: Day 3 Aghdaee, S. M., & Cavanagh, P. (2007). Temporal limits of long-range phase discrimination across the visual field. Vision Research 47, 2156-2163. Alvarez, G. A., & Cavanagh, P. (2005). Independent resources for attentional tracking in the left and right visual hemifields. Psychological Science 16, 637-643. Awh, E., & Pashler, H. (2000). Evidence for split attentional foci. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26, 834-846. Chakravarthi, R., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). Bilateral field advantage in visual crowding. Vision Research 49, 1638-1646. Kraft, A., et al. (2005). Interactions between task difficulty and hemispheric distribution of attended locations: implications for the splitting attention debate. Cognitive Brain Research 24, 19-32. Reardon, K. M., Kelly, J. G., & Matthews, N. (2009). Bilateral attentional advantage on elementary visual tasks. Vision Research 49, 692-702. Taya, S., Adams, W. J., Graf, E. W., & Lavie, N. (2009). The fate of task-irrelevant visual motion: Perceptual load versus feature-based attention. Journal of Vision 9, 1-10. Westheimer, G. (2003). Meridional anisotropy in visual processing: Implications for the neural site of the oblique effect. Vision Research 43, 2281-2289. Laterality x distracter interaction effect: F (4, 88) = 2.520, p = 0.047, partial η2 = 0.103 Laterality effect, distracters absent: F (2, 44) = 3.457, p = 0.040, partial η2 = 0.136 Distracter main effect: F (1.344, 29.566) = 13.146, p < 0.0005, partial η2 = 0.374 Laterality main effect : F (2, 36) = 17.304, p < 0.0005, partial η2 = 0.490 Day main effect: F (1, 18) = 38.032, p < 0.0005, partial η2 = 0.679 Laterality x Task x Day: F (2, 36) = 0.473, p = 0.627, partial η2 = 0.026 F (2,36) = 12.861, p < 0.0005, partial η2 = 0.417 http://denison.edu/~matthewsn/spacetimeattentionvss2010.html