Face Adaptation Depends on Seeing the Face

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Cross-modal attention influences auditory contrast sensitivity: Decreasing visual load improves auditory thresholds for amplitude- and frequency-modulated.
Advertisements

From: Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Interacting Roles of Attention and Visual Salience in V4
Long-Term Speeding in Perceptual Switches Mediated by Attention-Dependent Plasticity in Cortical Visual Processing  Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky 
Volume 87, Issue 4, Pages (August 2015)
Volume 27, Issue 7, Pages (April 2017)
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Michelle Lai, Ipek Oruç, Jason J.S. Barton  Cortex 
Natalia Zaretskaya, Andreas Bartels  Current Biology 
Efficient Receptive Field Tiling in Primate V1
Christopher C. Pack, Richard T. Born, Margaret S. Livingstone  Neuron 
Generalized Flash Suppression of Salient Visual Targets
Ian M. Finn, Nicholas J. Priebe, David Ferster  Neuron 
Independent Aftereffects of Attention and Motion
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Independent Binocular Rivalry Processes for Motion and Form
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
Volume 51, Issue 1, Pages (July 2006)
A Channel for 3D Environmental Shape in Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Volume 18, Issue 4, Pages (January 2017)
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Face Adaptation without a Face
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness in Human Extrastriate Cortex
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Contour Saliency in Primary Visual Cortex
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages (January 2007)
Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action  Paul Cisek,
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons
Volume 45, Issue 5, Pages (March 2005)
Eye Movement Preparation Modulates Neuronal Responses in Area V4 When Dissociated from Attentional Demands  Nicholas A. Steinmetz, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Normalization Regulates Competition for Visual Awareness
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Arielle Tambini, Nicholas Ketz, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
Phase Locking of Single Neuron Activity to Theta Oscillations during Working Memory in Monkey Extrastriate Visual Cortex  Han Lee, Gregory V. Simpson,
Category Selectivity in the Ventral Visual Pathway Confers Robustness to Clutter and Diverted Attention  Leila Reddy, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Gregor Rainer, Earl K Miller  Neuron 
Top-Down Modulation of Lateral Interactions in Early Vision
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Tuned Normalization Explains the Size of Attention Modulations
Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
Taosheng Liu, Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco  Neuron 
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Efficient Receptive Field Tiling in Primate V1
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Face Adaptation Depends on Seeing the Face Farshad Moradi, Christof Koch, Shinsuke Shimojo  Neuron  Volume 45, Issue 1, Pages 169-175 (January 2005) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.018

Figure 1 Face Adaptation under Dichoptic Viewing (A) Twelve observers were exposed to a specific antiface for 4 s in the adapting eye while viewing a rotating sphere of random dots in the other eye. Participants were instructed to continuously monitor and report the visibility of the face. After adaptation, they were asked to identify the subsequent face. In the nonrivalry condition (data not shown), the other image was blank. (B) Histograms of total duration of suppression of the face stimuli for four representative observers (top four histograms) and all twelve pooled observers. (C) Psychometric face identification curves after adaptation. The difference between the dashed and solid curves reflects the face-specific aftereffect. (D) Adaptation to an antiface decreased the detection threshold of the corresponding face compared to the threshold for unrelated faces in the nonrivalry condition, but not when the face was suppressed and invisible for more than 3 s. Partial suppression indicates trials in which the antiface was visible for more than 1 s. Neuron 2005 45, 169-175DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.018)

Figure 2 The Face Aftereffect Depends on the Duration of Predominance of the Antiface (A) We pooled the data of all 12 observers in experiment 1. A significant aftereffect occurs when the antiface is visible for more than 1 s. Error bars correspond to SEM. (B) The time dependence of the face aftereffect is parallel to the effect of the duration of physical exposure in the binocular setup where the antiface is always visible (experiment 2; five observers). The threshold change is normalized relative to the group mean of threshold change after 4 s visibility in each experiment (*p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001; one-tailed paired Student's t test). Neuron 2005 45, 169-175DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.018)

Figure 3 Orientation-Selective Adaptation Is Not Affected by Binocular Suppression (A) Seven observers were first exposed to a grid for 250 ms, followed by two slowly drifting gratings for 4 s in the adapting eye, while viewing a rotating sphere of random dots (rivalry condition) or blank screen (nonrivalry condition) in the other eye. Participants were instructed to continuously monitor and report the visibility of the grating. After adaptation, they were asked if the subsequent low-contrast grating was in the left or right visual field. (B) Adaptation to a particular orientation increased the detection threshold for the same orientation compared to the threshold for orthogonal orientation in both nonrivalry and invisible (suppressed >3 s) trials. In agreement with previous findings, the orientation of the stimulus can be suppressed from awareness (“Invisible”), without causing a reduction in the orientation-dependent aftereffect relative to control (“Nonrivalry”). Neuron 2005 45, 169-175DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.018)

Figure 4 The Face Aftereffect Is Reduced under High Attentional Load (A) Observers were asked to perform a 2-back memory task during 4 s of adaptation (high-attentional load condition; experiment 3). A stream of digits appeared at fixation. Observers were required to perform two tasks: the 2-back task during adaptation and the target detection task when the target face appeared. (B) Identification curves following adaptation to the corresponding antiface (dashed) versus an unrelated face (solid). The two curves are almost identical when observers are engaged in a highly attention-demanding visual (experiment 3; seven naive observers)—but not auditory (experiment 4; five naive observers)—task. In the “passive viewing” trials, observers were not required to carry out any interfering task during the adaptation. (C) Summary data comparing the reduction of the face aftereffect by binocular suppression and within and cross-modal attentional load. Neuron 2005 45, 169-175DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.018)