Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Advertisements

Mackenzie Weygandt Mathis, Alexander Mathis, Naoshige Uchida  Neuron 
Elizabeth V. Goldfarb, Marvin M. Chun, Elizabeth A. Phelps  Neuron 
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
René Quilodran, Marie Rothé, Emmanuel Procyk  Neuron 
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Enhanced Muscle Afferent Signals during Motor Learning in Humans
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Independent Aftereffects of Attention and Motion
Generating Coherent Patterns of Activity from Chaotic Neural Networks
Mismatch Receptive Fields in Mouse Visual Cortex
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Scale-Invariant Movement Encoding in the Human Motor System
Volume 55, Issue 3, Pages (August 2007)
Volume 64, Issue 4, Pages (November 2009)
Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Neuronal Correlates of Motor Performance and Motor Learning in the Primary Motor Cortex of Monkeys Adapting to an External Force Field  Chiang-Shan Ray.
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking Activity in Early Visual Cortex
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Annabelle C. Singer, Loren M. Frank  Neuron 
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action  Paul Cisek,
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
Action Selection and Action Value in Frontal-Striatal Circuits
Volume 74, Issue 4, Pages (May 2012)
Kevin Wood Bieri, Katelyn N. Bobbitt, Laura Lee Colgin  Neuron 
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
Human Dorsal and Ventral Auditory Streams Subserve Rehearsal-Based and Echoic Processes during Verbal Working Memory  Bradley R. Buchsbaum, Rosanna K.
Effects of Long-Term Visual Experience on Responses of Distinct Classes of Single Units in Inferior Temporal Cortex  Luke Woloszyn, David L. Sheinberg 
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism
Michael A. Silver, Amitai Shenhav, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Orienting Attention Based on Long-Term Memory Experience
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Short-Term Memory for Figure-Ground Organization in the Visual Cortex
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Jude F. Mitchell, Kristy A. Sundberg, John H. Reynolds  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (September 2000)
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Volume 65, Issue 4, Pages (February 2010)
Volume 80, Issue 1, Pages (October 2013)
James N. Ingram, Ian S. Howard, J. Randall Flanagan, Daniel M. Wolpert 
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Michael A. Silver, Amitai Shenhav, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories Hannah R. Sheahan, David W. Franklin, Daniel M. Wolpert  Neuron  Volume 92, Issue 4, Pages 773-779 (November 2016) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.017 Copyright © 2016 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Paradigm in which Planning and Execution of a Follow-Through Movement Were Factorially Controlled Participants made an initial movement from a starting location (bottom gray circle) to a central target (gray circle). During exposure trials, a velocity-dependent curl force field (force vectors shown as blue arrows for a typical straight line movement to the central target) was applied during this movement, and the field direction, clockwise (CW) or counter-clockwise (CCW), was determined by the secondary target location (at either +45° or −45° to the initial movement direction). A no follow-through group (top left) ended the movement at the central target, whereas the full follow-through group (bottom right) made a follow-through movement, thereby both planning and executing the follow through. For the execution-only group (bottom left), the secondary target only appeared late in the movement to the central target and they were required to follow through. Therefore, this group was prevented from planning the follow through prior to the initiation of their movement. For the planning-only group (top right), the secondary target disappeared late in the movement to the central target and they were required not to follow through. Therefore, this group could plan a follow through before the initiation of the movement, but did not execute it. In all groups, channel trials were used to assess learning and for these trials the secondary target was displayed from the start of the trial. The schematic only shows one of the four possible starting locations used in the experiment. Neuron 2016 92, 773-779DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.017) Copyright © 2016 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Planning Follow-Through Movements Reduces Interference between Opposing Fields (A) The kinematic error and (B) force adaptation for the full follow-through (blue), no follow-through (gray), execution-only (yellow), and planning-only (orange) groups. Data show mean ± SE across participants for pairs of blocks in the exposure phase (gray region) and for single blocks in the pre- and post-exposure phases. In (A), we show the mean (±SE) of the aftereffects to the right of the panel (separated for clarity). Neuron 2016 92, 773-779DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.017) Copyright © 2016 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Kinematics across the Groups for Different Phases of the Experiment Hand paths are shown to the central target from the four different starting positions. Paths shown as mean ± SE across participants, for last two blocks of pre-exposure (first column), the first two blocks (second column) and last (third column) two blocks of exposure, and the first two blocks of post-exposure (fourth column). The colors indicate the field direction (blue for CW and red for CCW). Neuron 2016 92, 773-779DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.017) Copyright © 2016 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Varying the Appearance Time of the Secondary Target Does Not Facilitate Adaptation in an Execution-Only Group (A) The kinematic error (normalized within this group) and (B) force adaptation (combining 0° and 180° early appearing channel trials). Data show mean ± SE across participants for pairs of blocks. In (A), we show the mean (±SE) of the aftereffects to the right of the panel. In (C), we show adaptation as a function of target appearance time for the second half of the blocks of exposure. Data show mean (±SE) of separate running averages performed for each subject, each with a 150 ms smoothing window at 100 appearance times equally spaced from 0.1 to 0.6 s. Neuron 2016 92, 773-779DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.017) Copyright © 2016 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions