Bettina Sorger, Joel Reithler, Brigitte Dahmen, Rainer Goebel 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Advertisements

Joshua J. Foster, Emma M. Bsales, Russell J. Jaffe, Edward Awh 
Decoding Sound and Imagery Content in Early Visual Cortex
Volume 28, Issue 6, Pages e4 (March 2018)
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Decoding Wakefulness Levels from Typical fMRI Resting-State Data Reveals Reliable Drifts between Wakefulness and Sleep  Enzo Tagliazucchi, Helmut Laufs 
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Rachel Ludmer, Yadin Dudai, Nava Rubin  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Volume 26, Issue 14, Pages (July 2016)
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Enhanced Muscle Afferent Signals during Motor Learning in Humans
Volume 21, Issue 19, Pages (October 2011)
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)
Representation of Object Weight in Human Ventral Visual Cortex
Huan Luo, Xing Tian, Kun Song, Ke Zhou, David Poeppel  Current Biology 
Sing-Hang Cheung, Fang Fang, Sheng He, Gordon E. Legge  Current Biology 
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
The Primate Cerebellum Selectively Encodes Unexpected Self-Motion
Scale-Invariant Movement Encoding in the Human Motor System
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Intrinsic and Task-Evoked Network Architectures of the Human Brain
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Human Cortical Neurons in the Anterior Temporal Lobe Reinstate Spiking Activity during Verbal Memory Retrieval  Anthony I. Jang, John H. Wittig, Sara.
Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns
Volume 26, Issue 7, Pages (April 2016)
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Modality-Independent Coding of Spatial Layout in the Human Brain
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Volume 81, Issue 5, Pages (March 2014)
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Decoding the Yellow of a Gray Banana
Volume 19, Issue 6, Pages (March 2009)
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)
Volume 19, Issue 3, Pages (February 2009)
Timing, Timing, Timing: Fast Decoding of Object Information from Intracranial Field Potentials in Human Visual Cortex  Hesheng Liu, Yigal Agam, Joseph.
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Andrew Clouter, Kimron L. Shapiro, Simon Hanslmayr  Current Biology 
Vahe Poghosyan, Andreas A. Ioannides  Neuron 
Volume 26, Issue 14, Pages (July 2016)
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory  Ignasi Sols, Sarah DuBrow,
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Cross-Modal Associative Mnemonic Signals in Crow Endbrain Neurons
Lysann Wagener, Maria Loconsole, Helen M. Ditz, Andreas Nieder 
Hippocampal-Prefrontal Theta Oscillations Support Memory Integration
Volume 34, Issue 4, Pages (May 2002)
Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology 
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Spatiotemporal Neural Pattern Similarity Supports Episodic Memory
Presentation transcript:

A Real-Time fMRI-Based Spelling Device Immediately Enabling Robust Motor- Independent Communication  Bettina Sorger, Joel Reithler, Brigitte Dahmen, Rainer Goebel  Current Biology  Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages 1333-1338 (July 2012) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022 Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2012 22, 1333-1338DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Letter Coding Scheme Combining three mental tasks, three task onset delays, and three task durations allows for encoding 3 × 3 × 3 = 27 distinct information units. When assigning these 27 combinations to 26 letters and the blank space (“–”), every character can be represented by a certain cognitive event leading to a unique dynamic brain response pattern. In order to encode, e.g., the letter “E,” a motor action has to be imagined, starting 10 s after the general trial onset and lasting for 30 s. Remarks/abbreviations are as follows: RTCs, reference time courses; ROIs, regions of interest; see Table S1 for mental tasks and ROIs used in the current study; see Movie S1 for brain activation patterns evoked by performing different mental tasks; curves shown are modeled RTCs derived from standard hemodynamic response functions (note, however, that individual RTCs were used for letter decoding [see Figure S3 for comparison of individual versus standard RTCs]). Current Biology 2012 22, 1333-1338DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Visually Guided Letter Encoding Help with Time Line In order to encode a specific letter, participants did not have to memorize the encoding parameters for a certain letter (i.e., mental task, onset delay, task duration). They only had to attend to the selected letter and to perform the particular mental task assigned to the respective row (indicated in the first column of each window) as long as the accordant letter cell was highlighted (for 10 s, 20 s, or 30 s). In order to encode, e.g., the letter “E,” no mental task has to be performed in the first 10 s following the onset of the letter encoding trial (passive). When the “E” cell gets highlighted, the participant starts performing “motor imagery” (active). As soon as the letter cell is no longer highlighted (after 30 s in this example), the participant stops and stays focusing on “E” for another 10 s (passive) until the whole letter encoding period is finished. During the subsequent resting period, in which none of the letter cells is highlighted, the participant is asked to select (and switch to) the next intended letter—remaining in a sort of “stand-by” mode, selecting the next mental task to be performed, and awaiting the next active letter encoding phase. Note that the conceptualization of our letter encoding display and the visual display invented in the context of the development of an EEG-based spelling device via event-related potentials (P300) [2] share common characteristics. Remarks are as follows: circles were not visible for participants; they are shown here in order to emphasize active (green) and passive (red) answer encoding phases when encoding the letter “E”; a dynamic version of the letter encoding help is provided as part of Movie S2.; note that hemodynamically based letter encoding can also be realized using exclusively auditory instructions (see Figure S4). Current Biology 2012 22, 1333-1338DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Real-Time Letter Decoding Group Results Averaged across all participants, the first letter choice of the automated decoding was correct in 82% of the cases (chance level: 3.7%). Considering the first two and three letter choices, the encoded letter was included in 95% and 100% of the cases. When regarding the main decoder output (“top three”-letter choices) and word/question context information, the experimenters were able to decipher the answers in 100% of the cases. Comparing the results obtained from answers to initial (bright colors) versus follow-up (dark colors) questions revealed that encoding performance remained stable throughout the MRI scanning session. Remarks are as follows: light gray bar areas represent the letter decoding accuracy based on prior letter choices; error bars indicate variance across participants (±SEM); see Figure S2 for overview of the automated letter decoding procedure. Current Biology 2012 22, 1333-1338DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Intrasession Mini-Conversations and Single-Trial Letter Decoding Results Participants (column 1) were asked an initial question (column 2) and encoded their answers letter-by-letter using the letter encoding technique. The automated letter decoder displayed its letter choices in ranked order (first three lines in each cell of column 3). After the experimenters had decided which word was most likely encoded (green characters in column 3), a follow-up question—containing this particular word (column 4)—was posed to and answered by the participant (column 5). Remarks are as follows: blue letters indicate correctly decoded letters; green letters form the word the experimenters decided for online; red letters represent incorrectly decoded letters; gray letters represent letter choices being less likely correct than the correct letter choice; participant 1 forgot to encode the first blank space in the initial run and added it at the end of that run; participant 5 forgot to encode two blank spaces; see Table S2 for participants' characteristics; see Figure S1 for single-trial multi-ROI time course data used for decoding the word “INDIA.” Current Biology 2012 22, 1333-1338DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions