Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(3):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Advertisements

Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Sexual Dimorphism in the Hoverfly Motion Vision Pathway
Representation of Object Weight in Human Ventral Visual Cortex
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Ingvars Birznieks, Richard M. Vickery  Current Biology 
Volume 97, Issue 4, Pages e6 (February 2018)
Volume 26, Issue 3, Pages (February 2016)
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Serial Dependence in the Perception of Faces
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e3 (December 2017)
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Brian D Corneil, Etienne Olivier, Douglas P Munoz  Neuron 
Kyoko Yoshida, Nobuhito Saito, Atsushi Iriki, Masaki Isoda 
The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Optic flow induces spatial filtering in fruit flies
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Humans Have an Expectation That Gaze Is Directed Toward Them
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept
Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Albert V. van den Berg, Jaap A. Beintema  Neuron 
Gaze and the Control of Foot Placement When Walking in Natural Terrain
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
Volume 37, Issue 3, Pages (February 2003)
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 
A Visual Sense of Number
Neuroscience: Tiny Eye Movements Link Vision and Attention
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans Eckart Zimmermann, David Burr, Maria Concetta Morrone  Current Biology  Volume 21, Issue 16, Pages 1380-1384 (August 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.014 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Spatial Specificity of Outward Adaptation of Double-Step Memory-Guided Saccades (A) Time course of events during tests trials. Subjects started each trial by fixating the fixation point (FP). After 1300–1500 ms, the secondary saccade target (T2) was shown for 1000 ms for the subject to memorize while keeping fixation. Both T2 and FP were then extinguished, and the primary saccade target (T1) was turned on, to which subjects saccaded immediately, followed by a second saccade to the remembered position of T2. VEP and HEP represent the vertical and horizontal eye movements. (B) Experimental setup. At the beginning of each test trial, the subject fixated the fixation point, FP1 for rightward saccades and FP2 for leftward saccades. For rightward saccades to T2, target T1 (black circle) was presented in the left part of the screen. For rightward saccades to T2, target T1 (colored symbols) was shown in one of six possible locations. Saccade target T2 (black diamond) was presented in the screen center, jittered around a range of 2°. (C) Error in second-saccade landing position in preadaptation trials for rightward (open square) and leftward saccades (black star), and error in second-saccade landing position in postadaptation trials for rightward (filled black square) and leftward saccades (colored symbols). The dashed line depicts the horizontal position of T2. Color coding of symbols is the same as in (B). Error bars in (C)–(E) indicate ±1 standard error of the mean. (D) Change of amplitude of the horizontal vector of the first saccade, expressed as the difference in average amplitude before and after adaptation. The black-outlined bar refers to the rightward saccade condition, and the colored-outline bars refer to the leftward conditions (code as in C). The dashed line represents the average change in amplitude for all leftward saccades. (E) As in (D), for the second saccade. Adaptation had clear effects on the second saccades in different directions for rightward and leftward saccades, resulting in similar landing positions. Current Biology 2011 21, 1380-1384DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Head-Centered versus Eye-Centered Spatial Specificity of Saccadic Adaptation (A) Time course of events during test trials (as in Figure 1A). (B–E) Sample landing positions of the second saccade to the memorized target, for pre- and postadaptation sessions for a single subject for the four experimental conditions (illustrated by schematics at the upper right of each panel). The dashed lines (green, red, and blue) indicate the three positions of the memory saccade target, which was jittered around a range of 2°. The color code of the points refers to the physical target position of that trial. The larger rectangles with error bars represent the mean landing positions, with ±1 standard error of the mean; small filled dots indicate individual saccadic postadaptation landing positions. Current Biology 2011 21, 1380-1384DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Allocentric versus Head-Centered Spatial Specificity of Saccadic Adaptation (A) Effect of adaptation on the amplitude of the horizontal saccadic vector of the first saccade in the double-step sequence, expressed as the difference in average amplitude before and after adaptation. Error bars represent the standard error of the sample mean. (B) Effect of head turn on adaptation-induced horizontal saccade amplitude changes of the second saccade in the double-step sequence. Horizontal saccades were adapted with the head facing the screen center, and test trials were collected with the head turned 9° leftward (purple) or rightward (orange). The adaptation remained spatiotopic with head turns. Error bars represent the standard error of the sample mean. (C) Experimental setup. Saccade adaptation was induced with the head in a straight-ahead position in all sessions. In head-turn sessions, saccade amplitudes were then tested either for leftward (purple arrow) or rightward (orange arrow) head turns. For both head turns, the spatiotopic and the retinotopic conditions were tested. The leftward head turn causes the retinotopic sequence to become also craniotopic: in both adaptation and test conditions, the nose points to the fixation point at the beginning of the session, and the sequence is identical from there. Current Biology 2011 21, 1380-1384DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.014) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions