Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Advertisements

Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Interacting Roles of Attention and Visual Salience in V4
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Linking Electrical Stimulation of Human Primary Visual Cortex, Size of Affected Cortical Area, Neuronal Responses, and Subjective Experience  Jonathan.
Value Representations in the Primate Striatum during Matching Behavior
Ian M. Finn, Nicholas J. Priebe, David Ferster  Neuron 
Volume 84, Issue 1, Pages (October 2014)
Braden A. Purcell, Roozbeh Kiani  Neuron 
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
A Neural Signature of Divisive Normalization at the Level of Multisensory Integration in Primate Cortex  Tomokazu Ohshiro, Dora E. Angelaki, Gregory C.
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Perceptual Learning and Decision-Making in Human Medial Frontal Cortex
Michael L. Morgan, Gregory C. DeAngelis, Dora E. Angelaki  Neuron 
An Optimal Decision Population Code that Accounts for Correlated Variability Unambiguously Predicts a Subject’s Choice  Federico Carnevale, Victor de Lafuente,
Visual Search and Attention
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Unreliable Evoked Responses in Autism
Hedging Your Bets by Learning Reward Correlations in the Human Brain
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Joshua I. Sanders, Balázs Hangya, Adam Kepecs  Neuron 
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Contrast Adaptation and Representation in Human Early Visual Cortex
Torben Ott, Simon Nikolas Jacob, Andreas Nieder  Neuron 
Nicholas J. Priebe, David Ferster  Neuron 
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Peter Kok, Janneke F.M. Jehee, Floris P. de Lange  Neuron 
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Eye Movement Preparation Modulates Neuronal Responses in Area V4 When Dissociated from Attentional Demands  Nicholas A. Steinmetz, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
James M. Jeanne, Tatyana O. Sharpee, Timothy Q. Gentner  Neuron 
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Guilhem Ibos, David J. Freedman  Neuron 
Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Timescales of Inference in Visual Adaptation
Daniel E. Winkowski, Eric I. Knudsen  Neuron 
Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Jude F. Mitchell, Kristy A. Sundberg, John H. Reynolds  Neuron 
Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Paying Attention to the Details of Attention
Kristy A. Sundberg, Jude F. Mitchell, John H. Reynolds  Neuron 
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Supratim Ray, John H.R. Maunsell  Neuron 
Taosheng Liu, Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco  Neuron 
Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Presentation transcript:

Attentional Enhancement via Selection and Pooling of Early Sensory Responses in Human Visual Cortex  Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner  Neuron  Volume 72, Issue 5, Pages 832-846 (December 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Signal Detection Theory, Sensitivity Enhancement, and Efficient Selection (A) Contrast-discrimination performance depends on the contrast-response function (black curve) and variability of response. Stimuli of contrast c and c+Δc evoke neural responses with mean amplitude R(c) and R(c+Δc), respectively. Across trials, presentation of the same stimuli elicits slightly different responses, depicted by the distributions on the ordinate that have standard deviation σ. Behavioral sensitivity (d′, see equation) is theorized to be equal to the difference between the responses to the two stimuli (green arrow) divided by σ. (B–D) Top panels show possible effects of attention on the contrast-response functions. Bottom panels illustrate distributions of responses for two stimuli of different contrasts, showing how focal attention may affect stimulus discriminability. (B) Focal attention increases the slope of the contrast-response function. (C) Focal attention reduces the trial-to-trial variability in sensory neural responses. (D) Focal attention selects the relevant sensory signal (red arrow), so irrelevant information is ignored. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Behavioral Protocol Observers performed a two-interval forced-choice contrast-discrimination task. On each trial, four gratings appeared in two temporal intervals (Stim1 and Stim2). Only one grating (target) had a slightly higher contrast in one of the intervals. A response cue (green arrow in Resp) appearing after stimulus offset indicated the location of the target. Observers reported the interval in which the target was higher in contrast. There were two kinds of attentional cues: (A) focal cue, a single arrow indicating the correct target location with 100% validity; and (B) distributed cue, four arrows indicating that the target was equally likely to appear in any quadrant. The protocol thus defined four stimulus cue combinations: (1) focal cue target, red arrow; (2) focal cue nontarget, green arrows; (3) distributed cue target, blue arrow; and (4) distributed cue nontarget, purple arrow. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Contrast-Discrimination Performance Contrast-discrimination thresholds for the focal (red) and distributed cue (blue) conditions (mean ± SEM across observers; some error bars are smaller than symbols) are plotted as a function of stimulus contrast. Thick curves are best fit of Equations 2–4. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Testing Response Enhancement (A) Example fMRI response time courses from one observer's V1. Colors indicate different stimulus cue combinations (see legend at bottom right). Shading indicates stimulus contrast; darker colors correspond to lower contrasts. Error bars, SEM over repeated trials. (B) Example contrast-response functions, corresponding to the response time courses in (A). Continuous curves, best fit of Equation 3. (C) Contrast-response functions for each visual area and stimulus cue combination averaged across observers (mean ± 1 SEM) for each visual area. See also Figure S1. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Testing Sensory Noise Reduction Left column shows contrast-discrimination functions (mean ± SEM across observers) for distributed (blue, A) and focal (red, C and E) cues. Right column illustrates V1 contrast-response functions (mean ± 1 SEM across observers) for distributed (blue, B) and focal (red, D and F) cues. Contrast-response functions are plotted on log-linear axis in the large panels and on linear-linear axis in the insets. The smooth curves are fits of the model, with parameter values listed in each box (σ, sensory noise; b, baseline response). Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 6 Noise Estimates and Baseline Responses Inferred from Model Fits (A) Ratio of sensory noise on distributed cue trials (σd) to focal cue trials (σf). (B) Difference between baseline on focal cue trials (bf) and distributed cue trials (bd). Error bars, ± 1 SEM across observers. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 7 Selection Model (A) Sensory response distributions. Each panel plots simulated response distributions, the proportion of trials on which a given response was evoked, for each stimulus location. Blue indicates first stimulus interval. Orange illustrates second stimulus interval (that included the contrast increment at the target location). Left panel shows focal cue trials. Right panel indicates distributed cue trials. Insets are schematics of stimulus displays and attention cues (see Figure 2). (B) Max-pooling operation. Sensory responses to each stimulus interval were pooled across locations according to the max-pooling rule (best-fit k = 68.08). (C) Readout distributions after pooling across locations. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 8 Comparison of Sensory Noise Reduction and Efficient Selection (A) Contrast-discrimination functions (data replotted from Figure 3) fit with the selection model (continuous curves), constrained by the average V1 contrast-response functions (not shown, but see Figure 4). The two free parameters, k (max-pooling parameter, Equation 1) and σ (sensory noise standard deviation), were constrained to be the same for the focal and distributed cue conditions. (B) Contrast-discrimination functions fit with the sensitivity model, again constrained by the V1 contrast-response functions. Noise standard deviation (σ) was constrained to be 50% smaller for focal than distributed cue trials. (C) Changes in noise standard deviation (σ) for focal and distributed cue trials estimated by the selection (left) and the sensitivity (right) models. Error bars, ± 1 SEM across observers. See also Figure S2. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 9 Testing Prediction of Selection Model that High-Contrast Distracters Disrupt Behavioral Performance (A) Contrast discrimination thresholds (mean ± SEM across observers; some error bars are smaller than symbols) for focal (red) and distributed (blue) cue conditions as a function of distracter condition (see legend). (B) Ratio of thresholds averaged across pedestal contrast. Error bars, SEM across observers. Neuron 2011 72, 832-846DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.025) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions