Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
A Sensorimotor Role for Traveling Waves in Primate Visual Cortex
Advertisements

Evidence for Surface-Based Processing of Binocular Disparity
Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1
Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
Volume 60, Issue 5, Pages (December 2008)
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Cursive Writing with Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements
Volume 27, Issue 14, Pages e5 (July 2017)
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Michael L. Morgan, Gregory C. DeAngelis, Dora E. Angelaki  Neuron 
Neural Correlates of Knowledge: Stable Representation of Stimulus Associations across Variations in Behavioral Performance  Adam Messinger, Larry R. Squire,
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Karl R Gegenfurtner, Jochem Rieger  Current Biology 
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Prefrontal Cortex Activity Related to Abstract Response Strategies
The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Jingping P. Xu, Zijiang J. He, Teng Leng Ooi  Current Biology 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept
MT Neurons Combine Visual Motion with a Smooth Eye Movement Signal to Code Depth-Sign from Motion Parallax  Jacob W. Nadler, Mark Nawrot, Dora E. Angelaki,
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Higher-Order Figure Discrimination in Fly and Human Vision
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Donald E. Mitchell, Jan Kennie, Diane Kung  Current Biology 
Population Responses to Contour Integration: Early Encoding of Discrete Elements and Late Perceptual Grouping  Ariel Gilad, Elhanan Meirovithz, Hamutal.
Color and Luminance Contrasts Attract Independent Attention
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
A Visual Sense of Number
Valerio Mante, Vincent Bonin, Matteo Carandini  Neuron 
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Maxwell H. Turner, Fred Rieke  Neuron 
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis David C Burr, Michael J Morgan, M.Concetta Morrone  Current Biology  Volume 9, Issue 20, Pages 1207-1209 (October 1999) DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)80028-7 Copyright © 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Illustration of the stimuli used in this experiment. The motion sequence comprised two frames, each four cycles of horizontal sinusoidal gratings of 0.1 cycles per degree, subtending 60 × 40° when viewed from 30 cm away. The gratings were displayed on a monitor (Barco Calibrator), surrounded by white card (100 × 150 cm) floodlit to a similar mean luminance as the display screen. The grating was displayed for 100 msec in a given random chosen phase, then for another 100 msec with phase displaced ± 90° (100 msec was chosen because it gives a very strong motion signal). Subjects were required to report whether the motion was upward or downward. One frame (presented either first or second) had a fixed contrast throughout the experimental session (fixed-contrast frame), while the contrast of the other (the test frame) varied. Threshold was measured using the QUEST procedure to vary contrast over the region near threshold, then calculating threshold as the contrast at which 75% of responses were correct [14]. Thresholds were determined by fitting cumulative Gaussian functions to the percentage correct results. Stimuli were generated by a framestore under computer control (Cambridge Research Systems, VSG) and displayed on a Barco Calibrator monitor at 200 frames/sec, with mean luminance 20 cd/m2. During ‘normal’ conditions, observers initiated the motion sequence by pressing a button. For the saccadic conditions, observers saccaded from the fixation target on the left to a spot that was turned on 20° to the right. The eye movements were recorded by a computer-monitored infrared recorder (HVS Image), and used to trigger the motion sequence (in practice 10–15 msec after saccade initiation). After each trial, the saccade was displayed on the same monitor to the observer, together with the time of initiation of the motion sequence. If the saccade was not adequate (if it was of insufficient amplitude, it included a ‘corrective saccade’ or the stimulus was triggered too late) the observer aborted that trial. Current Biology 1999 9, 1207-1209DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(00)80028-7) Copyright © 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Contrast sensitivity (inverse of contrast thresholds) for discriminating the direction of a two-frame motion sequence as a function of the contrast of the fixed frame. Green circles refer to measurements under normal viewing conditions, blue triangles to those during saccades. Graphs are shown for each observer (DCB, MCM and MJM). The graphs on the left (filled symbols) depict results for motion sequences in which the test frame was presented first, those on the right (open symbols) for when it was second. The green curves are the geometric means of these two conditions in normal viewing. The fact that the curve passes very near the data points in both conditions shows that thresholds were very similar under normal viewing whether the test frame was first or second. During saccades, however, the graphs were very different for the two conditions. Sensitivity was much higher for when the test frame was presented second than when it was presented first, particularly at high contrasts. At some contrasts, sensitivity was sometimes higher during saccadic than normal viewing for this condition. The blue curves are saccadic sensitivities that are predicted if the first and second stimuli were suppressed independently of each other. These curves were derived from the average normal sensitivity estimates by displacing the green data points along both logarithmic axes, illustrated by the blue arrows (corresponding to a multiplicative scaling). The resultant displacement vector is illustrated by the black arrows. The multiplicative values (horizontal and vertical, respectively) of saccadic suppression used for the displacements were: DCB 4.0, 2.1; MCM 8.1, 2.9; MJM 9.0, 4.0. The dashed green and blue horizontal lines show contrast sensitivity when the contrast of the two frames was yoked. Current Biology 1999 9, 1207-1209DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(00)80028-7) Copyright © 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd Terms and Conditions