Face Pareidolia in the Rhesus Monkey

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Piercing of Consciousness as a Threshold-Crossing Operation
Advertisements

Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Volume 60, Issue 5, Pages (December 2008)
Single-Neuron Correlates of Atypical Face Processing in Autism
Neuronal Correlates of Metacognition in Primate Frontal Cortex
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Joshua P. Bassett, Thomas J. Wills, Francesca Cacucci  Current Biology 
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Volume 83, Issue 3, Pages (August 2014)
Lexical Influences on Auditory Streaming
Grid Cells Encode Local Positional Information
Clayton P. Mosher, Prisca E. Zimmerman, Katalin M. Gothard 
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Beauty Requires Thought
Volume 97, Issue 4, Pages e6 (February 2018)
Miguel P. Eckstein, Kathryn Koehler, Lauren E. Welbourne, Emre Akbas 
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
A Channel for 3D Environmental Shape in Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex
Volume 18, Issue 4, Pages (January 2017)
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Miguel P. Eckstein, Kathryn Koehler, Lauren E. Welbourne, Emre Akbas 
Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms of Overgeneralization in Anxiety
Marian Stewart Bartlett, Gwen C. Littlewort, Mark G. Frank, Kang Lee 
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Contextual Feedback to Superficial Layers of V1
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Medial Axis Shape Coding in Macaque Inferotemporal Cortex
Decoding the Yellow of a Gray Banana
The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Effects of Long-Term Visual Experience on Responses of Distinct Classes of Single Units in Inferior Temporal Cortex  Luke Woloszyn, David L. Sheinberg 
Grid Cells Encode Local Positional Information
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Grid and Nongrid Cells in Medial Entorhinal Cortex Represent Spatial Location and Environmental Features with Complementary Coding Schemes  Geoffrey W.
Timing, Timing, Timing: Fast Decoding of Object Information from Intracranial Field Potentials in Human Visual Cortex  Hesheng Liu, Yigal Agam, Joseph.
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Robust Selectivity to Two-Object Images in Human Visual Cortex
David Pitcher, Vincent Walsh, Galit Yovel, Bradley Duchaine 
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Volume 92, Issue 2, Pages (October 2016)
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Category Selectivity in the Ventral Visual Pathway Confers Robustness to Clutter and Diverted Attention  Leila Reddy, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Volume 17, Issue 15, Pages (August 2007)
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
The Perception and Misperception of Specular Surface Reflectance
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Implicit Vocabulary Learning during Sleep Is Bound to Slow-Wave Peaks
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, Stefano Panzeri  Current Biology 
Impaired Associative Learning with Food Rewards in Obese Women
Volume 80, Issue 1, Pages (October 2013)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
Neurophysiology of the BOLD fMRI Signal in Awake Monkeys
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Presentation transcript:

Face Pareidolia in the Rhesus Monkey Jessica Taubert, Susan G. Wardle, Molly Flessert, David A. Leopold, Leslie G. Ungerleider  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 16, Pages 2505-2509.e2 (August 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.075 Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2017 27, 2505-2509.e2DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.075) Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Methods (A) Examples of the three stimulus types used (from left to right: unfamiliar female monkeys, illusory faces, and non-face objects). The non-face objects were selected from the public domain on the basis that they matched the examples of illusory faces for object content. (B) The results of the human experiment. Here, the rows represent individual subject data (n = 10) and columns represent the 45 images comprising the stimulus set. Importantly, none of the non-face objects was rated as being “face-like” (>100) on a 200-point scale (Mnon-face objects = 5.24; SEM = 0.45). Two pairwise contrasts confirmed that the non-face objects had a significantly smaller average score than either the monkey faces (p < 0.01, η2 = 0.99) or illusory faces (p < 0.01, η2 = 0.99). (C) The trial procedure for the three conditions of interest in the monkey experiment. Each trial consisted of three time periods: fixation, free viewing, and reward after successful trial completion or time out after trial aborts. Current Biology 2017 27, 2505-2509.e2DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.075) Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Experimental Results (A) Bar graph indicates the average proportion of time spent looking at stimuli as a function of condition (error bars = ±SEM). We found the expected advantage for monkey faces over objects, together with the hypothesized advantage for illusory faces over objects. We computed the average mean difference in each condition (monkey faces LT subtracted from illusory faces LT [I − M]; non-face objects LT subtracted from illusory faces LT [I − O]; non-face objects LT subtracted from monkey faces LT [M − O]) and performed a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA (p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.86) to confirm that illusory face paired with monkey face trials elicited the smallest stimulus preference (paired t tests, two-tailed; [I − M] versus [I − O], p < 0.01, η2 = 0.97; [I − M] versus [M − O], p = 0.016, η2 = 0.80; [I − O] versus [M − O], p = 0.300 η2 = 0.24). (B) Bar graph demonstrating the distribution of first fixations in the three conditions of interest (the number of first fixations is expressed as a proportion of the total number of trials in each condition; error bars = ±SEM). An analysis of the first fixation data indicated that, in trials where monkey faces were presented with non-face objects, subjects fixated the monkey faces first and more often (p = 0.01, η2 = 0.92). There was a similar advantage for illusory faces over non-face objects (p = 0.01, η2 = 0.96). As with the LT data, this analysis also revealed a significant preference for illusory faces over monkey faces (p = 0.01, η2 = 0.92). See also Figure S1. Current Biology 2017 27, 2505-2509.e2DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.075) Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Fixations Calibrated in Degrees of Visual Angle and Superimposed on Stimuli (A) Average number of fixations (≥150 ms) in two-dimensional density plots (three examples from each stimulus type; top row, monkey faces; middle row, illusory faces; bottom row, non-face objects). Data were normalized to each subject’s maximum fixation count and then averaged across subjects before being smoothed and superimposed on the corresponding stimulus for illustration using MATLAB’s surf function with interpolated shading. Unsmoothed data for every stimulus, together with individual subject maps (before averaging), are available in the Figure S3. (B) The range of grand r values as a function of stimulus type. After vectorizing the normalized fixation count data for each subject, we cross-correlated across subjects. This process yielded ten r values that were then averaged together to yield a single “grand r value” per stimulus. The lower r values evident for the non-face objects reflect the greater variance among individual subjects. (C) Classifier performance as a function of subject; the classifier was trained with 93.33% of the data (i.e., 14 out of 15 illusory face/non-face pairs) and tested with the remaining content-matched pair. Chance performance is 50%. See also Figures S2 and S3. Current Biology 2017 27, 2505-2509.e2DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.075) Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions