Motion-Based Analysis of Spatial Patterns by the Human Visual System

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Advertisements

The “Flash-Lag” Effect Occurs in Audition and Cross-Modally
A Motion Direction Map in Macaque V2
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Vincent Jacob, Julie Le Cam, Valérie Ego-Stengel, Daniel E. Shulz 
Volume 6, Issue 5, Pages e5 (May 2018)
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Rhythmic Working Memory Activation in the Human Hippocampus
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Josh H. McDermott, Eero P. Simoncelli  Neuron 
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Volume 21, Issue 19, Pages (October 2011)
Ian M. Finn, Nicholas J. Priebe, David Ferster  Neuron 
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
Andrea Benucci, Robert A. Frazor, Matteo Carandini  Neuron 
Michael L. Morgan, Gregory C. DeAngelis, Dora E. Angelaki  Neuron 
Visual Search and Attention
Volume 55, Issue 3, Pages (August 2007)
Binocular Disparity and the Perception of Depth
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Spatiotemporal Elements of Macaque V1 Receptive Fields
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Nicholas J. Priebe, David Ferster  Neuron 
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Coding of Natural Scenes in Primary Visual Cortex
Origin and Function of Tuning Diversity in Macaque Visual Cortex
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Suppression without Inhibition in Visual Cortex
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Greg Schwartz, Sam Taylor, Clark Fisher, Rob Harris, Michael J. Berry 
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Alon Poleg-Polsky, Huayu Ding, Jeffrey S. Diamond  Cell Reports 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Local and Global Contrast Adaptation in Retinal Ganglion Cells
Shin'ya Nishida, Alan Johnston  Current Biology 
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex
Does Neuronal Synchrony Underlie Visual Feature Grouping?
Receptive Fields of Disparity-Tuned Simple Cells in Macaque V1
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Local Origin of Field Potentials in Visual Cortex
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Gregor Rainer, Earl K Miller  Neuron 
Higher-Order Figure Discrimination in Fly and Human Vision
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
Hippocampal-Prefrontal Theta Oscillations Support Memory Integration
Søren K. Andersen, Steven A. Hillyard, Matthias M. Müller 
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Motion Adaptation and the Velocity Coding of Natural Scenes
Neurophysiology of the BOLD fMRI Signal in Awake Monkeys
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Motion-Based Analysis of Spatial Patterns by the Human Visual System Shin'ya Nishida  Current Biology  Volume 14, Issue 10, Pages 830-839 (May 2004) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044

Figure 1 Stimulus Configuration (A) The letter pattern presented behind the slits. Two sets of letters move in the opposite directions (as indicated by arrows). A panel with diagonal stripes, presented below, is a space-time (X-T) plot of a line of the lower-letter set. (B) The pattern presented with stationary multiple slits. The condition of interslit separation was 32′. In this stationary figure, letter identification is almost impossible because of the presence of slits. However, letter movement significantly improves the visibility of the letters. A demonstration movie is available at http://www.brl.ntt.co.jp/people/nishida/demo. Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)

Figure 2 Letter Identification Was Better When the Letters Moved Behind the Slits Than When They Were Stationary (A) The percent of trials in which the observer could correctly identify the two letters is plotted as a function of slit separation. The exposure duration was 320 ms (64 frame). The results of four observers are shown. The error bars indicate the standard errors across blocks. An open cross symbol indicates the data obtained when the order of motion sequence was randomized (for S.N., H.K., and M.H., only). (B) The letter identification performance (in terms the threshold slit interval for which the proportion of correct trials was 50%) is plotted as a function of the number of frames (bottom abscissa)/exposure duration (top abscissa). The effects of letter movements were evident even when the exposure duration was less than 100 ms. Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)

Figure 3 The Spatiotemporal Structure of Multislit Display (A) A 3D space-time (X-Y-T) plot of an original unsampled moving letter “A.” (B) The Fourier spectrum of (A), which is lying on a single plane passing through the origin. (C) A 3D space-time plot of a moving letter sampled by a vertical slit array. (D) The Fourier spectrum of (C). Due to the slit sampling, aliasing noise components appear on the planes parallel to the signal plane. (E) A 2D Fourier spectrum of the sampled image (top view of [D]). Dashed lines indicate the spatial Nyquit frequency (1/2w, where w is the slit interval). Gray area indicates the passband of the optimal nondirection-selective spatiotemporal low-pass filter that segregates signal from noise. The signal components higher than the Nyquist frequency cannot be recovered. (F) The passband of a direction-selective filter that segregates signal from noise. The signal components higher than the Nyquist frequency can be recovered. Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)

Figure 4 Direction-Selective Masking of Letter Identification by Random-Dot Noise (A) A slit array with 50% contrast noise. A space-space (X-Y) plot, as well as space-time (X-T) plots for the noise moving in the same direction as or that opposite to the letter movement. (B) The percent of correct trials as a function of noise contrast. The noise movement was in the same direction (filled circles) or in the opposite direction (open squares). A dashed line indicates the performance measured without noise. The speed of the noise movement was matched with the letter movement (6.7°/s). The exposure duration was 320 ms. The interslit interval was determined for each observer in a pilot test as the value that gave ∼50% identification with the 25% contrast noise moving in the same direction (28′, 28′, 26′, and 22′ for S.N., H.K., M.H., and I.M., respectively). Letter identification was impaired more effectively when the noise moved in the same direction. Demonstration movies are available at http://www.brl.ntt.co.jp/people/nishida/demo. (C) Speed tuning of the noise masking. The letter identification performance (in terms the threshold slit interval for which the proportion of correct trials was 50%) is plotted as a function of the noise speed. The speed of letter movement (6.7°/s) is indicated by an arrow. The results indicate that the noise masking is broadly tuned to speed. Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)

Figure 5 Direction-Selective Adaptation of Letter Identification The percent of correct trials measured after adaptation to moving random lines is plotted as a function of slit interval. Letter identification was worse when the adapted direction was the same as the letter movement (filled circle) than when it was opposite (open square). The data measured without adaptation are shown for comparison (open cross, replotted from Figure 2A). Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)

Figure 6 The Estimation of the Map of Perceived Spatial Frequencies (A) An example of random-frequency mask (window open rate: 50%). (B) An example of filtered image. (C) An example of a filtered image shown through slits (interslit interval: 32′). (D) The spatial frequency map (correlogram) of letter identification obtained without slits and with slits. Each map is based on 6000 trials (12,000 responses) by three observers. The percent correct was 60.7% for the no-slit condition and 51.1% for the slit condition. For each condition, the top panel is the frequency map for the full 32 × 32 subband windows (from zero to eight cycles, corresponding to the first quadrant of the mask shown in [A]). Spatial frequency is expressed in terms of slit sampling cycle (one cycle = 1/32′). The color indicates the correlation coefficient of the state of each subband and the observers' response. Green indicates that the pattern components in this subband had positive contribution to the letter identification (i.e., somehow visible). Since such subbands cluster in the lower spatial frequency range, the frequency map up to two cycles is magnified in the bottom panel. In the right panel (slit condition), a dashed vertical line indicates the Nyquist frequency of slit sampling (a half of the sampling frequency). The correlogram indicates that the frequency components higher than this Nyquist frequency were perceived by the observers. This theoretically requires direction-based pattern interpolation. Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)

Figure 7 Possible Roles of the Two Stages of Early Motion Processing in Spatiotemporal Pattern Interpolation One is direction-selective linear filtering and the other is motion energy computation, whose neural correlates may be direction-selective simple cells and complex cells in V1, respectively. The first stage response to a multislit stimulus (A) can be simulated by convolving the space-time plot of the input image by spatiotemporally slanted filters (shown in insets of [B] and [C]). The two filters differ in phase by 90° (quadrature pair). As shown by (B) and (C), the filtering is able to interpolate continuously moving edges. The second-stage response (D) is the squared sum of (B) and (C), in which the fine edge structure is blurred out. This suggests that the visual system may use V1 simple cells, but not complex cells, for spatiotemporal pattern interpolation. Current Biology 2004 14, 830-839DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.044)