\nathan\RaterBias.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Gene-environment interaction models
Advertisements

Bivariate analysis HGEN619 class 2007.
Fitting Bivariate Models October 21, 2014 Elizabeth Prom-Wormley & Hermine Maes
Estimating “Heritability” using Genetic Data David Evans University of Queensland.
Path Analysis Danielle Dick Boulder Path Analysis Allows us to represent linear models for the relationships between variables in diagrammatic form.
Multivariate Analysis Nick Martin, Hermine Maes TC21 March 2008 HGEN619 10/20/03.
Introduction to Multivariate Analysis Frühling Rijsdijk & Shaun Purcell Twin Workshop 2004.
Multivariate Genetic Analysis: Introduction(II) Frühling Rijsdijk & Shaun Purcell Wednesday March 6, 2002.
Extended sibships Danielle Posthuma Kate Morley Files: \\danielle\ExtSibs.
When Measurement Models and Factor Models Conflict: Maximizing Internal Consistency James M. Graham, Ph.D. Western Washington University ABSTRACT: The.
Multiple raters March 7 th, 2002 Boulder, Colorado John Hewitt.
Univariate Analysis in Mx Boulder, Group Structure Title Type: Data/ Calculation/ Constraint Reading Data Matrices Declaration Assigning Specifications/
Multivariate Analysis Hermine Maes TC19 March 2006 HGEN619 10/20/03.
Biometrical Genetics Pak Sham & Shaun Purcell Twin Workshop, March 2002.
David M. Evans Sarah E. Medland Developmental Models in Genetic Research Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics Oxford United Kingdom Twin Workshop Boulder.
Path Analysis Frühling Rijsdijk SGDP Centre Institute of Psychiatry King’s College London, UK.
Introduction to Multivariate Genetic Analysis Kate Morley and Frühling Rijsdijk 21st Twin and Family Methodology Workshop, March 2008.
Path Analysis Frühling Rijsdijk. Biometrical Genetic Theory Aims of session:  Derivation of Predicted Var/Cov matrices Using: (1)Path Tracing Rules (2)Covariance.
Raw data analysis S. Purcell & M. C. Neale Twin Workshop, IBG Colorado, March 2002.
Social interaction March 7 th, 2002 Boulder, Colorado John Hewitt.
LECTURE 16 STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING.
Phenotypic multivariate analysis. Last 2 days……. 1 P A A C C E E 1/.5 a cecae P.
MEASUREMENT MODELS. BASIC EQUATION x =  + e x = observed score  = true (latent) score: represents the score that would be obtained over many independent.
A B C D (30) (44) (28) (10) (9) What is a gene? A functional group of DNA molecules (nucleotides) that is responsible for the production of a protein.
Standard genetic simplex models in the classical twin design with phenotype to E transmission Conor Dolan & Janneke de Kort Biological Psychology, VU 1.
Karri Silventoinen University of Helsinki Osaka University.
Institute of Psychiatry King’s College London, UK
Cholesky decomposition May 27th 2015 Helsinki, Finland E. Vuoksimaa.
Univariate modeling Sarah Medland. Starting at the beginning… Data preparation – The algebra style used in Mx expects 1 line per case/family – (Almost)
Longitudinal Modeling Nathan Gillespie & Dorret Boomsma \\nathan\2008\Longitudinal neuro_f_chol.mx neuro_f_simplex.mx jepq6.dat.
Gene-Environment Interaction & Correlation Danielle Dick & Danielle Posthuma Leuven 2008.
Achievement & Ascription in Educational Attainment Genetic & Environmental Influences on Adolescent Schooling François Nielsen.
G Lecture 81 Comparing Measurement Models across Groups Reducing Bias with Hybrid Models Setting the Scale of Latent Variables Thinking about Hybrid.
Mx modeling of methylation data: twin correlations [means, SD, correlation] ACE / ADE latent factor model regression [sex and age] genetic association.
Model building & assumptions Matt Keller, Sarah Medland, Hermine Maes TC21 March 2008.
Introduction to Genetic Theory
Introduction to Multivariate Genetic Analysis Danielle Posthuma & Meike Bartels.
Developmental Models/ Longitudinal Data Analysis Danielle Dick & Nathan Gillespie Boulder, March 2006.
March 7, 2012M. de Moor, Twin Workshop Boulder1 Copy files Go to Faculty\marleen\Boulder2012\Multivariate Copy all files to your own directory Go to Faculty\kees\Boulder2012\Multivariate.
Multivariate Genetic Analysis (Introduction) Frühling Rijsdijk Wednesday March 8, 2006.
Extended Pedigrees HGEN619 class 2007.
Gene-Environment Interaction
Multivariate Analysis
Bivariate analysis HGEN619 class 2006.
Univariate Twin Analysis
Questions What are the sources of error in measurement?
Introduction to Multivariate Genetic Analysis
Re-introduction to openMx
Gene-Environment Interaction & Correlation
CHAPTER 5 MEASUREMENT CONCEPTS © 2007 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.
MRC SGDP Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience
Path Analysis Danielle Dick Boulder 2008
Chapter 15: Correlation.
Genetics of qualitative and quantitative phenotypes
Univariate modeling Sarah Medland.
Studying Twins and Adoptions Can Help Assess the Importance of Genes and Environment Genetics.
GxE and GxG interactions
Pak Sham & Shaun Purcell Twin Workshop, March 2002
Sarah Medland faculty/sarah/2018/Tuesday
Getting more from multivariate data: Multiple raters (or ratings)
BOULDER WORKSHOP STATISTICS REVIEWED: LIKELIHOOD MODELS
Multivariate Genetic Analysis
Multitrait Scaling and IRT: Part I
Multivariate Genetic Analysis: Introduction
Rater Bias & Sibling Interaction Meike Bartels Boulder 2004
Biological approach and OCD
Netherlands Twin Register (NTR)
Presentation transcript:

\nathan\RaterBias

Observer Ratings: Dealing with rater bias Nathan Gillespie Meike Bartels John Hewitt

Multiple raters Rather than measure individual’s phenotypes directly, we often rely on observer ratings Example Parent & teacher ratings of children Problem How do you handle bias which is a tendency of a rater to over or underestimate scores consistently Response Bias - stereotyping, different normative standards, response style Projection Bias - psychopathology of the parent influences his/her judgement of the behavior of the child e.g. several studies suggest that depression in mothers may lead to overestimating their children’s symptoms Rater bias can inflate C How to disentangle child’s phenotype from rater bias?

Example of multiple rater data: Problem behavior Data from Netherlands Twin Registry Questionnaires ages 3, 5, 7, 10 & 12 - maternal & paternal ratings ages 7, 10, and 12 - teacher ratings ages 12, 14, 16 - self report Internalizing - Anxious/Depressed, Somatic Complaints & Withdrawn subscales Externalizing - Aggressive & Rule Breaking subscales. Mother's & father's ratings of aggressive behaviour in boys at 12 yrs The Child Behavior Checklist - behavioral & emotional problems of 4- to 18yr olds . 120 problem items. 3-point scale, based on the occurrence of behavior preceding 6 mths 0=not true, 1=somewhat/sometimes true, 2=very true/often true. 2 broadband scales Internalizing (INT) & Externalizing (EXT) Internalizing - Anxious/Depressed, Somatic Complaints & Withdrawn subscales. Externalizing - Aggressive & Rule Breaking subscales.

Multiple raters Analysis of parent / teacher ratings depends on assumptions YOU make! 1. Biometric model – agnostic i.e. treat data as assessing different phenotypes. Good if mothers and fathers rate / observe kids in different situations! 2. Psychometric model – assume there is a common phenotype assessed by both parents + specific effects uniquely observed by each each parent 3. Rater bias model – Ratings of a child’s phenotype modeled as a function of child’s phenotype + bias introduced by the rater

1. Biometric model Model mother's and father's ratings agnostically The mother's and father's ratings may be correlated but for unspecified reasons. Mothers' and fathers' ratings are assessing different phenotypes. - ratings are taken across different situations - mums and dad don't have a common understanding of the behavioural description In this case we would simply model the ratings in terms of a standard bivariate analysis We could model mother's and father's ratings agnostically i.e. make no assumptions. The mother's and father's ratings may be correlated but for unspecified reasons. This model treats the mothers' and fathers' ratings are assessing different phenotypes. This is plausible, particular if the ratings are taken across different situations, or perhaps mum and dad don't have a common understanding of the behavioural description. In this case we would simply model the ratings in terms of a standad bivariate analysis.

1. Biometric model A C E A C E Mother's ratings Father's ratings Treat parental ratings as separate phenotypes A C E A C E δc11 δc21 δe11 δe21 δa21 δa22 δc22 δe22 δa11 δc11 δe21 Mother's ratings Father's ratings

The Mx script Script Cholesky1.mx Data: TAD.dat Task Fix error & calculate standardized variance components

Variance-covariance matrices in Mx MZ (A+C+E | A+C_ A+C | A+C+E ) ; DZ (A+C+E | H@A+C_ H@A+C | A+C+E ) ;

Polychoric correlations Variance Decomposition Mother's ratings Father's ratings A .59 .58 C .23 .28 E .18 .14 1. 2. 3. 4. 1. Mother T1 1.00 2. Father T1 .72 3. Mother T2 .71 .57 4. Father T2 .73 -2LL df 3243.16 1816

2. Psychometric Model More restrictive assumptions There is a common phenotype which is being assessed by mothers and fathers AND There is a component of the each parent's ratings which assesses an independent aspect of the children's behaviour. Mother and father ratings would therefore correlate because they are making assessments based on shared observations and shared understanding of the behavioural descriptions

Reliable trait variance T1 Reliable trait variance T2 2. Psychometric Model 1 1 , ½ E C A A C E c c e a a e Reliable trait variance T1 Reliable trait variance T2 Mum’s rating T1 Father’s rating T1 Father’s rating T2 Mum’s rating T2 ef cf af af cf ef Cm Am Em Em Cm Am em cm am am cm em 1 , ½ Cm Am Em Em Cm Am 1 1 , ½ 1

+ Total variance for an individual + + x x x x MRT1 FRT1 1 = a  A + c  C + e  E x Am Af am 0 0 af x Cm Cf cm 0 0 cf x Em Ef em 0 0 ef x + +

The Mx script Script Psychometric1.mx Data TAD.dat Task Fix error & note variance components

Variance-covariance matrices in Mx MZ (G+S+F | G+S_ G+S | G+S+F) + L * (A+C+E | A+C_ A+C | A+C+E ) * L' ; DZ (G+S+F | H@G+S_ H@G+S | G+S+F) + L * (A+C+E | H@A+C_ H@A+C | A+C+E ) * L' ;

Variance decomposition Mother's ratings Father's ratings Latent factor A .42 .39 C .14 .13 E .03 Residuals Ares .17 .19 Cres .09 .14 Eres .11 -2LL df 3243.16 1816

Rater Bias Model Even more restrictive Assumes that there is a common phenotype which is being assessed by mothers and fathers Phenotype is again a function of three latent factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a non-shared environmental factor (E). Rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, & residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors.

Reliable trait variance T1 Reliable trait variance T2 Rater Bias Model 1 1 , ½ E C A A C E c c e a a e Reliable trait variance T1 Reliable trait variance T2 1 1 1 1 mother rating T1 father rating T1 mother rating T2 father rating T2 rm rf rm rf Rm T1 Rf T1 Rm T2 Rf T1 bf bm bm bf Mum’s bias Dad’s bias

+ Total variance for an individual + x x x MRT1 FRT1 1 = a  A + c  C + e  E x bm 0 0 bf x Bm Bf Rm Rf rm 0 0 rf + x

The Mx script Script Raterbias1.mx Data TAD.dat Task Fix error & note variance components

Variance-covariance matrices in Mx MZ (S+F | S_ S | S+F) + L * (A+C+E | A+C_ A+C | A+C+E ) * L' ; DZ (S+F | S_ S | S+F) + L * (A+C+E | H@A+C_ H@A+C | A+C+E ) * L' ;

Variance decomposition Mother's ratings Father's ratings Latent factor A .53 .51 C .05 E .00 Residuals Rater Bias .23 .29 Eres .19 .15 -2LL df 3257.37 1818

Results: Model comparison -2LL df BIC Cholesky 3243.16 1816 -1171.22 Psychometric Rater Bias 3257.37 1818 -1167.19

Conclusions 1. Rater bias, if not controlled for, ends up in shared environment 2. Besides rater bias, rater specific views are a source of rater disagreement > multiple rater design valuable 3. Psychometric model provides most information on sources of rater disagreement

Sibling Interaction / Rater Contrast Path s implies an interaction between phenotypes Twin I Twin 2 1 1 vs 0.5 s E A C e c a Egmond 2005 Workshop on Multivariate Modelling of Genetic Data

Sibling Interaction Social Interaction between siblings (Carey, 1986; Eaves, 1976) Behaviour of one child has a certain effect on the behavior of his or her co-twin: Cooperation - behavior in one twin leads to like-wise behavior in the co- twin Competition - increased behavior in one twin leads to decreased behavior in co-twin Egmond 2005 Workshop on Multivariate Modelling of Genetic Data

Rater Contrast Behavioural judgment / rating of one child of a twin pair is NOT independent of the rating of the other child of the twin pair. Rate compares the twins behaviour against one another The behaviour of the one child becomes a ‘standard’ by the which the behaviour of the other co-twin is judged / rated. Parents may either stress the similarities or differences between the children

Effects of rater contrast Phenotypic cooperation / positive rater contrast Mimics the effects of shared environment Increases the variance of more closely related individuals (var MZ >> var DZ) Phenotypic competition / negative rater contrast Mimics the effects of non-additive genetic variance Increases the variance of more closely related individuals the least (var MZ << var DZ)

Numerical Illustration a2=0.5, d2=0, c2=0, e5=0.5 S = 0; cooperation >> s = 0.5; competition >> s = -0.5 Social interactions cause the variance of the phenotype to depend on the degree of relationship of the social actors MZ DZ Unrelated Var Cov r None 1 .50 .25 Cooperation 3.11 2.89 .93 2.67 2.33 .88 2.22 1.78 .80 Competition 1.33 .44 .33 -.67 -.38 -1.78 -.80

P1 = sP2 + aA1 + cC1 + eE1 P2 = sP1 + aA2 + cC2 + eE Contrast Effect P1 = sP2 + aA1 + cC1 + eE1 P2 = sP1 + aA2 + cC2 + eE Twin I Twin 2 1 1 vs 0.5 s E A C e c a

Contrast Effect P1 = sP2 + aA1 + cC1 + eE1 P2 = sP1 + aA2 + cC2 + eE2 a c e 0 0 0 0 0 0 a c e

Matrix expression y = By + Gx y – By = Gx (I-B) y = Gx (I-B)-1 (I-B)y = (I-B)-1 Gx y = (I-B)-1 Gx

Mx Begin Matrices; B full 2 2 ! constrast effect End Matrices; Begin Algebra; P = (I-B)~; End Algebra

Variance – Covariance Matrix MZs P & ( A + C + E | A + C_ A + C | A + C + E) / DZs P & ( A + C + E | H@A + C_ H@A + C | A + C + E) /

The Mx script Script: Contrast.mx Data: TAD.dat

Consequences for variation & covariation Basic model X1 X2 x x s P1 P2 s P1 = sP2 + xX1 P2 = sP1 + xX2

In matrices P1 P2 X1 X2 P1 P2 0 s s 0 P1 P2 x 0 0 x X1 X2 = + In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors. y = By + Gx

Matrix expression y = By + Gx y – By = Gx (I-B) y = Gx (I-B)-1 (I-B)y = (I-B)-1 Gx y = (I-B)-1 Gx In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors.

Matrix expression y = (I-B)-1 Gx where (I-B) is Which has determinant: (1*1-s*s) = 1-s2 , so (I-B)-1 is 1 0 0 1 0 s s 0 1 -s -s 1 - = In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors. 1 s s 1 1 1-s2 @

Matrix expression Variance-covariance matrix for P1 and P2 Σ { yy’} = { (I-B)-1 Gx} { (I-B)-1 Gx}’ = (I-B)-1 G Σ {xx’} G’ (I-B)-1’ where Σ {xx’} is covariance matrix of the x variables In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors.

Matrix expression We want to standardize variables X1 and X2 to have unit variance and correlation r, therefore Σ {xx’} = P1 P2 s X1 X2 x In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors. 1 r r 1

To compute the covariance matrix recall that… G = 1 s s 1 1 1-s2 @ (I-B)-1 = In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors. 1 r r 1 Σ {xx’} =

To compute the covariance matrix recall that… 1 + 2sr + s2 r+2s + rs2 r+2s + rs2 1 + 2sr + s2 x2 (1-s2)2 @ Σ { yy’} = In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors.

The effects of sibling interaction on variance and covariance components between pairs of relatives Source Variance Covariance Additive genetic ω(1+2sra+s2)a2 ω(ra+2s+ras2)a2 Dominance ω(1+2srd+s2)d2 ω(rd+2s+rds2)d2 Shared env ω(1+2src+s2)c2 ω(rc+2s+rcs2)c2 Non-shared env ω(1+2sre+s2)e2 ω(re+2s+res2)e2 where ω = scalar 1/(1-s2)2 In the Rater Bias model the phenotypes of the twins are a function of three common factors underlying the ratings of both mothers and fathers: a genetic factor (A), a shared environmental factor (C), and a nonshared environmental factor (E). In addition to these three common factors, rater-specific factors are modeled: a maternal rater bias factor, a paternal rater bias factor, and residual (unreliability) factors affecting each rating. The influence of the common factors is assumed to be independent of the maternal and paternal rater bias and unreliability factors.

Rater Bias Influence shared environmental variance! Independent of zygosity Response Bias - stereotyping, different normative standards, response style Projection Bias - Psychopathology of the parent influences his/her judgement of the behavior of the child e.g. several studies suggest that depression in mothers may lead to overestimating their children’s symptomology

Parental Disagreement Multiple raters Rather than measure individual’s phenotypes directly, we rely on observer ratings. Example: Parent & teacher ratings of children’s behaviour Problem: How to disentangle child’s phenotype from rater bias? Rater bias can influence C (independent of zygosity) Parental Disagreement Rater bias / error (e.g. response style, different normative standards) Mother or father provide specific information - distinct situations, parent-specific relation with child

Agreements versus Disagreements Rater Bias Parental ratings Agreements versus Disagreements