Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1
Advertisements

Visual Influences on Echo Suppression
Joshua J. Foster, Emma M. Bsales, Russell J. Jaffe, Edward Awh 
A Human Extrastriate Area Functionally Homologous to Macaque V4
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Volume 20, Issue 10, Pages R431-R435 (May 2010)
Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Visual Motion and the Perception of Surface Material
Selective Attention in an Insect Visual Neuron
There's more to magic than meets the eye
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
Nimish Khanna, Yan Hu, Andrew S. Belmont  Current Biology 
Visual Cortex: The Eccentric Area Prostriata in the Human Brain
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System
Effects of Anxiety on Spontaneous Ritualized Behavior
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Adaptive Surround Modulation in Cortical Area MT
Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Visual motion interferes with lexical decision on motion words
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Vision Guides Selection of Freeze or Flight Defense Strategies in Mice
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Working memory without consciousness
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Elementary motion detectors
Optic flow induces spatial filtering in fruit flies
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Magic and cognitive neuroscience
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Group Behaviour: Leadership by Those in Need
Volume 14, Issue 3, Pages (February 2004)
Volume 17, Issue 20, Pages R877-R878 (October 2007)
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Attention Reorients Periodically
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Claudia Lunghi, Uzay E. Emir, Maria Concetta Morrone, Holly Bridge 
The Perception and Misperception of Specular Surface Reflectance
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
A Visual Sense of Number
Volume 22, Issue 5, Pages (March 2012)
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception
Dynamic Displacement Vector Interacts with Tactile Localization
Maria J.S. Guerreiro, Lisa Putzar, Brigitte Röder  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Nimish Khanna, Yan Hu, Andrew S. Belmont  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals Bo Hu, David C. Knill  Current Biology  Volume 20, Issue 10, Pages R436-R437 (May 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.053 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental set up and results. (A) Subjects viewed a grating pattern co-aligned optically with the top of a cube held by subjects' right hand against a fronto-parallel table. A 12° aperture surrounding the grating was rendered binocularly 10 mm above the visual pattern. After viewing the pattern for 3500 ms, subjects reported the perceived direction of grating motion (see Supplemental Information). (B) When moving a grating along the direction indicated by the green arrow, subjects saw the motion of the grating in that direction, whereas a second observer saw it moving unambiguously along the grating normal direction (red arrow). (C–E) Subjects' perceived motion directions (relative to the grating normal direction) as a function of hand movement direction. The top row illustrates the stimuli used in corresponding plots. (C) A typical subject's data in trials with circular apertures in Experiment 1. Seven subjects showed similar results (see Figure S1). (D) The same subject's data in trials with square apertures reflect multi-stable motion percepts. The results varied across subjects (see Figure S1). (E) One subject's data in Experiment 2. Seven of the eight subjects showed the same results. One subject reported perceived motion in the direction of hand movement (see Figure S2). (F) Mean and SEM of the relative influence of the kinesthetic signal. A value of 1 reflects full kinesthetic capture (see Supplemental Information). The influence was close to 1 when the aperture was circular, showing that the perceived motion was largely determined by the kinesthetic signals. The influence was smaller and the variability greater when the aperture was square, because of subjects' mixed strategies in motion judgment. The influence was close to 0, indicating vision dominance, in the delay condition — the apparent small non-zero influence being due almost entirely to one subject who reported hand movement direction in all trials (see Figure S2). Current Biology 2010 20, R436-R437DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.053) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions