Spatiochromatic Properties of Natural Images and Human Vision

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
9-7:Congruent and Similar Figures Congruent Figures Similar Figures.
Advertisements

Dilations in the Coordinate Plane
Preview of Calculus.
McGraw-Hill/Irwin Copyright © 2010 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved. Chapter 6 Continuous Random Variables.
Estimation. Rounding  The simplest estimation technique is to round.  This works very well on formulas where all the values can be reduced to one significant.
Autonomous Robots Vision © Manfred Huber 2014.
Light Years and Parsecs
Computer Graphics CC416 Lecture 04: Bresenham Line Algorithm & Mid-point circle algorithm Dr. Manal Helal – Fall 2014.
Date of download: 6/27/2016 Copyright © 2016 SPIE. All rights reserved. Flow chart of the imaging processing. See Sec. 2 for details. Figure Legend: From:
Chapter 6 Continuous Random Variables Copyright © 2014 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
CS262 – Computer Vision Lect 4 - Image Formation
Math Module 2 Lines.
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
Copyright © 2007 Dan Nettleton
DATA ANALYSIS IN CHEMISTRY
Mean Shift Segmentation
Fitting Curve Models to Edges
Click the mouse or press the spacebar to continue.
Martin D Bootman, Michael J Berridge  Current Biology 
High-Resolution Laser Scanning Reveals Plant Architectures that Reflect Universal Network Design Principles  Adam Conn, Ullas V. Pedmale, Joanne Chory,
Coding of Cognitive Magnitude
What Image Properties Regulate Eye Growth?
Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys
Jamie L. Baldwin Fergus, Sönke Johnsen, Karen J. Osborn 
With Age Comes Representational Wisdom in Social Signals
Volume 22, Issue 13, Pages (July 2012)
A Unique Advantage for Giant Eyes in Giant Squid
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Single-Cell Analysis of Growth in Budding Yeast and Bacteria Reveals a Common Size Regulation Strategy  Ilya Soifer, Lydia Robert, Ariel Amir  Current.
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
Apparent Subdiffusion Inherent to Single Particle Tracking
Microtubule Structure at 8 Å Resolution
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Binocular Disparity and the Perception of Depth
Hedging Your Bets by Learning Reward Correlations in the Human Brain
Gene-Expression Variation Within and Among Human Populations
Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis
Evolutionary Conditions for the Emergence of Communication in Robots
Modes of Diffusion of Cholera Toxin Bound to GM1 on Live Cell Membrane by Image Mean Square Displacement Analysis  Pierre D.J. Moens, Michelle A. Digman,
Cascade Models of Synaptically Stored Memories
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Cortical Mechanisms of Smooth Eye Movements Revealed by Dynamic Covariations of Neural and Behavioral Responses  David Schoppik, Katherine I. Nagel, Stephen.
Microtubule Structure at 8 Å Resolution
Quantitative Reasoning
Karl R Gegenfurtner, Jochem Rieger  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 1, Pages 1-9 (January 2015)
Volume 15, Issue 9, Pages (September 2007)
Volume 5, Issue 4, Pages e4 (October 2017)
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Benjamin Scholl, Daniel E. Wilson, David Fitzpatrick  Neuron 
Kevin Wood Bieri, Katelyn N. Bobbitt, Laura Lee Colgin  Neuron 
Jamie L. Baldwin Fergus, Sönke Johnsen, Karen J. Osborn 
Anton Arkhipov, Peter L. Freddolino, Klaus Schulten  Structure 
Alon Poleg-Polsky, Huayu Ding, Jeffrey S. Diamond  Cell Reports 
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Benjamin Scholl, Daniel E. Wilson, David Fitzpatrick  Neuron 
Volume 85, Issue 6, Pages (December 2003)
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Visual Ecology: Coloured Fruit is What the Eye Sees Best
Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment
End-Stopping and the Aperture Problem
Mechanical Communication Acts as a Noise Filter
A Visual Sense of Number
Arati Sridharan, Chetan Patel, Jit Muthuswamy 
George D. Dickinson, Ian Parker  Biophysical Journal 
A way of selectively degrading colour constancy demonstrates the experience dependence of colour vision  Eli Brenner, Frans W. Cornelissen  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Spatiochromatic Properties of Natural Images and Human Vision C.A. Párraga, T. Troscianko, D.J. Tolhurst  Current Biology  Volume 12, Issue 6, Pages 483-487 (March 2002) DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00718-2 Copyright © 2002 Cell Press Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Decomposition of an Original Image into L, M, and S Cone-Sampled Images, and the Derivation from These of the Three Final Representations: Luminance, Red-Green Chromatic, and Blue-Yellow Chromatic (A) The analysis for a picture similar to those used in previous work. (B) The same analysis for a picture of a red tomato on a background of green leaves. Note that the luminance and chromatic representations have been scaled in order to maximize the available dynamic range. In fact, the red-green representation in (A) actually has much less amplitude than shown. In (B), the variability of color and brightness within the background of leaves is removed, making the uniform red tomato pop out. Note that the divisive term in the calculation of the red-green and blue-yellow representations means that the pixel values range between −1 and 1, while the scaling factor for the luminance representation is essentially arbitrary. Current Biology 2002 12, 483-487DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00718-2) Copyright © 2002 Cell Press Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Examples of the Luminance and Chromatic Spectral Slopes of Our Natural Scenes Circles correspond to the luminance, triangles correspond to the red-green signals, and squares correspond to the blue-yellow signals. The Fourier amplitude (averaged across orientation) is shown as a function of spatial frequency for the luminance and chromatic representations of the outdoor image with various kinds of foliage (Figure 1A) and the tomato image (Figure 1B). When logarithmic coordinates are used on both axes, the data fall on straight lines whose slopes are called the spectral slope (α), which usually has a value in the range −0.7 to −1.6. Different y scales are used for the luminance data (left scale) compared to the chromatic data (right scale), because of the different scaling in the pixel values of the two kinds of representation. (A) The plot shows no difference between the luminance and chromatic slopes. (B) The plot shows a steeper slope for the red-green image than for the blue-yellow and luminance images. Current Biology 2002 12, 483-487DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00718-2) Copyright © 2002 Cell Press Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 The Ratio of the Spectral Slopes of the Luminance and Chromatic Representations, for Different Image Types, as a Function of the Percentage of Pixels Signaling the Presence of a “Red” Object (A and B) The background always consisted of green foliage. The data plotted on the left correspond to pictures of scenery without a strong presence of red objects (normal scenery: distant landscapes or closeups of foliage), and their αlum/αchrom slope ratio near unity is consistent with previous findings [2]. The squares correspond to scenes with no presence of “red” pixels (i.e., they are plotted on the y axis). The line in (A) represents the approximate data trend and reflects the fact that the average αlum/αchrom slope ratio for normal scenery is about 1.1 (see Table S1). Table S1 shows the numerical means and standard deviations of the slope ratios. Note that there is a tendency for the slope ratio to fall with increasing proportion of red content for the red-green images (A), but not for the blue-yellow images against the proportion of blue content (B). A high red content indicated the presence of fruit or a similar object. Current Biology 2002 12, 483-487DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00718-2) Copyright © 2002 Cell Press Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 The Relationship between the Ratio of the Spectral Slopes and the Distance from the Camera to the Main Object in Each Scene, Assuming a 13.6° Total Subtense The squares represent “distant landscape” scenes, i.e., scenes in which objects (trees, rocks, bushes, etc.) were numerous and their location was further away than 50 m. The line represents the data trend and reflects the fact that average values of αlum/αchrom slope ratio are equal to 1.0 for these landscapes. The log x axis was chosen to best show the geometrical relationship between the variables. Distances were estimated from the real size of the object (in cm), the size of the object on the final picture (in pixels), and the subtended angle of our pictures. The optimal viewing distance (where the slope ratio is around 0.78) is therefore approximately 40 cm. It could be conservatively argued that this calculation suggests that the optimal viewing distance is of the order of magnitude of typical primate grasping distance. Since we are only making order-of-magnitude claims here, the trend-line was drawn by eye. Current Biology 2002 12, 483-487DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00718-2) Copyright © 2002 Cell Press Terms and Conditions