Volume 70, Issue 2, Pages (April 2011)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 65, Issue 6, Pages (March 2010)
Advertisements

Volume 63, Issue 3, Pages (August 2009)
Davide Nardo, Valerio Santangelo, Emiliano Macaluso  Neuron 
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Volume 47, Issue 6, Pages (September 2005)
Lior Shmuelof, Ehud Zohary  Neuron 
Neurodegenerative Diseases Target Large-Scale Human Brain Networks
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Frontal Cortex and the Discovery of Abstract Action Rules
The Neural Basis of Body Form and Body Action Agnosia
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Marcus Grueschow, Rafael Polania, Todd A. Hare, Christian C. Ruff 
Sing-Hang Cheung, Fang Fang, Sheng He, Gordon E. Legge  Current Biology 
Rajeev D.S. Raizada, Russell A. Poldrack  Neuron 
Predicting Value of Pain and Analgesia: Nucleus Accumbens Response to Noxious Stimuli Changes in the Presence of Chronic Pain  Marwan N. Baliki, Paul.
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e6 (April 2018)
Volume 63, Issue 3, Pages (August 2009)
Perceptual Learning and Decision-Making in Human Medial Frontal Cortex
Michael L. Morgan, Gregory C. DeAngelis, Dora E. Angelaki  Neuron 
Volume 53, Issue 6, Pages (March 2007)
Volume 79, Issue 4, Pages (August 2013)
Volume 50, Issue 2, Pages (April 2006)
Volume 70, Issue 2, Pages (April 2011)
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Elizabeth J. Coulthard, Parashkev Nachev, Masud Husain  Neuron 
Selective Entrainment of Theta Oscillations in the Dorsal Stream Causally Enhances Auditory Working Memory Performance  Philippe Albouy, Aurélien Weiss,
Neural Correlates of Visual Working Memory
Jack Grinband, Joy Hirsch, Vincent P. Ferrera  Neuron 
Talia Konkle, Aude Oliva  Neuron  Volume 74, Issue 6, Pages (June 2012)
Integration of Touch and Sound in Auditory Cortex
Between Thoughts and Actions: Motivationally Salient Cues Invigorate Mental Action in the Human Brain  Avi Mendelsohn, Alex Pine, Daniela Schiller  Neuron 
Volume 25, Issue 11, Pages (June 2015)
Volume 54, Issue 6, Pages (June 2007)
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Distributed Neural Systems for the Generation of Visual Images
The Functional Neuroanatomy of Object Agnosia: A Case Study
Lior Shmuelof, Ehud Zohary  Neuron 
Volume 62, Issue 6, Pages (June 2009)
Volume 81, Issue 5, Pages (March 2014)
Olaf Blanke, Mel Slater, Andrea Serino  Neuron 
Cosimo Urgesi, Salvatore M. Aglioti, Miran Skrap, Franco Fabbro  Neuron 
Michael S. Beauchamp, Kathryn E. Lee, James V. Haxby, Alex Martin 
The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain
Absolute Coding of Stimulus Novelty in the Human Substantia Nigra/VTA
Neurological and Robot-Controlled Induction of an Apparition
Volume 63, Issue 5, Pages (September 2009)
Uri Hasson, Orit Furman, Dav Clark, Yadin Dudai, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
Volume 42, Issue 2, Pages (April 2004)
Integration of Local Features into Global Shapes
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Cortical Motion Deafness
High-Resolution fMRI Reveals Laminar Differences in Neurovascular Coupling between Positive and Negative BOLD Responses  Jozien Goense, Hellmut Merkle,
Feeling numbness for someone else's finger
Volume 68, Issue 1, Pages (October 2010)
Cerebral Responses to Change in Spatial Location of Unattended Sounds
Orienting Attention Based on Long-Term Memory Experience
Brain Mechanisms for Extracting Spatial Information from Smell
Manuela Piazza, Philippe Pinel, Denis Le Bihan, Stanislas Dehaene 
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Michael S. Beauchamp, Kathryn E. Lee, James V. Haxby, Alex Martin 
Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment
Christian J. Fiebach, Jesse Rissman, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
Predicting Value of Pain and Analgesia: Nucleus Accumbens Response to Noxious Stimuli Changes in the Presence of Chronic Pain  Marwan N. Baliki, Paul.
Patterns of fMRI Activity Dissociate Overlapping Functional Brain Areas that Respond to Biological Motion  Marius V. Peelen, Alison J. Wiggett, Paul E.
Taosheng Liu, Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco  Neuron 
Volume 36, Issue 1, Pages (September 2002)
Yuko Yotsumoto, Takeo Watanabe, Yuka Sasaki  Neuron 
The Sound of Actions in Apraxia
Presentation transcript:

Volume 70, Issue 2, Pages 363-374 (April 2011) Multisensory Mechanisms in Temporo-Parietal Cortex Support Self-Location and First- Person Perspective  Silvio Ionta, Lukas Heydrich, Bigna Lenggenhager, Michael Mouthon, Eleonora Fornari, Dominique Chapuis, Roger Gassert, Olaf Blanke  Neuron  Volume 70, Issue 2, Pages 363-374 (April 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Visual and Tactile-Robotic Stimulation (A and B) Visual stimuli. In the body conditions, participants were shown a video of a wooden rod with a stimulation sphere (in red) that moved vertically along the midline of the virtual person's back (A). During the control conditions, the video showed only the moving rod and the stimulator (B). (C) Robotic stimulator installed on the scanner bed. The tactile stimulation of the participant's back was performed by a custom-made robotic device generating the same movement profile for the body and the control conditions. An ultrasonic motor placed at the level of the feet actuated the stimulation sphere over a rack-and-pinion mechanism. Motion was transmitted over a guided fiberglass rod, which held the stimulation sphere over a compliant blade in order to follow the participant's back with constant pressure. (D) The robotic device (stroking sphere in red) was placed between the two custom-made mattresses (in gray; the standard mattress is removed from the sliding scanner bed). (E) Participant (outside the scanner) placed on the two mattresses and the robotic device. Neuron 2011 70, 363-374DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Self-Location Manipulation Graphic representation of the experimentally induced changes in self-location and perspective in the Up- and Down-group. The position of the human bodies represents the experienced position as indicated by the self-location task (mental ball dropping). The labels on the trousers indicate the experimental conditions. The direction of the experienced first-person perspective (as measured through questionnaires) is represented by the direction of the feet and nose, as well as the black arrows (pointing upward or downward). In both perspective groups, the body/synchronous condition leads to a drift in self-location toward the virtual body, but in opposite directions depending on the experienced perspective. (A) Thus, participants that had the impression of looking upward at the virtual body (Up-group) had increased response times (RTs) in the MBD task during the synchronous as compared to the asynchronous stroking condition (represented by a blue line), indicating an elevation of self-location. (B) Participants that had the impression of looking downward at the virtual body from an elevated perspective (Down-group) had decreased RTs in the MBD task during the synchronous as compared to the asynchronous stroking condition (represented by a blue line) indicating a lowering of self-location. The drift in self-location occurred in the direction of the experienced perspective (black arrows). RTs for the MBD task are plotted for each group as a function of the factors Object and Stroking. Orange bars represent the synchronous stroking conditions and the red bars the asynchronous stroking conditions. Asterisks indicate significant differences. Error bars indicate standard error. Note the differences between synchronous and asynchronous stroking conditions only in the body conditions (not in the control conditions) and in opposite directions between Up- and Down-group. The MR-scanner is depicted only for illustration purposes (participants were not asked to estimate the position of the scanner bed). Neuron 2011 70, 363-374DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Questionnaire Scores Self-identification with the virtual body (Q3) and mislocalization of touch toward the virtual body (Q5) are stronger during the synchronous stroking condition in both the Up-group and Down-group. Orange bars indicate ratings in the synchronous stroking condition. Red bars indicate ratings in the asynchronous stroking condition. Asterisks indicate significant differences. Error bars indicate standard error. See also Table S1. Neuron 2011 70, 363-374DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 TPJ Activity and Self-Location TPJ activity in the body conditions is shown for the Up- and the Down-group. In both groups, the magnitude of the BOLD response was lower in conditions with high self-location as quantified by the MBD task (synchronous stroking in the Up-group; asynchronous stroking in the Down-group) as compared to conditions with lower estimated self-location (asynchronous stroking in the Up-group; synchronous stroking in the Down-group). (A) (higher panel) Left TPJ activation centered on pSTG. The lower panel shows the BOLD response at the left TPJ as a function of Perspective and Stroking during the body conditions. (B) Activation in left and right TPJ and left superior postcentral gyrus. (C) (higher panel) Right TPJ activation centered on pSTG. The lower panel shows the BOLD response at the right TPJ as a function of Perspective and Stroking during the body conditions. The pattern of the left TPJ BOLD response was the same as found at the right TPJ. (D) Activation of left and right posterior middle and inferior temporal gyri. See also Table S2. Asterisks indicate significant differences. Error bars indicate standard error. Neuron 2011 70, 363-374DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Brain Damage in Patients with Abnormal Self-Location Due to Out-of-Body Experience (A) Brain damage and results of lesion overlap analysis in nine patients with OBEs due to focal brain damage is shown. Maximal lesion overlap centers at the right TPJ at the angular gyrus (red). Overlap color code ranges from violet (one patient) to red (seven patients). Note that only one patient suffered from damage to the left TPJ. (B) Voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) of focal brain damage leading to OBEs. The violet-to-red cluster shows the region that VLSM analysis associated statistically with OBEs as compared to control patients. The color-code indicates significant Z-Scores (p < 0.05; FDR-corrected) of the respective voxels showing maximal involvement of the right TPJ, including the right pSTG, angular gyrus, and middle temporal gyrus. (C) Self-location and the TPJ. Comparison between the area reflecting experimentally induced changes in self-location in healthy participants at the right TPJ using fMRI (red) and the area reflecting pathologically induced changes in self-location in neurological patients with OBEs using VLSM (blue). See also Table S3. Neuron 2011 70, 363-374DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions