Coding of Cognitive Magnitude

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Bram-Ernst Verhoef, Rufin Vogels, Peter Janssen  Neuron 
Advertisements

A Sensorimotor Role for Traveling Waves in Primate Visual Cortex
A visual sense of number
Interacting Roles of Attention and Visual Salience in V4
One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIP
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Bram-Ernst Verhoef, Rufin Vogels, Peter Janssen  Neuron 
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Volume 64, Issue 3, Pages (November 2009)
Volume 72, Issue 4, Pages (November 2011)
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Jason A. Cromer, Jefferson E. Roy, Earl K. Miller  Neuron 
Sam Norman-Haignere, Nancy G. Kanwisher, Josh H. McDermott  Neuron 
Martin O'Neill, Wolfram Schultz  Neuron 
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Volume 97, Issue 4, Pages e6 (February 2018)
Neural Correlates of Knowledge: Stable Representation of Stimulus Associations across Variations in Behavioral Performance  Adam Messinger, Larry R. Squire,
Visual Search and Attention
Complementary Roles for Primate Frontal and Parietal Cortex in Guarding Working Memory from Distractor Stimuli  Simon Nikolas Jacob, Andreas Nieder  Neuron 
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Hedging Your Bets by Learning Reward Correlations in the Human Brain
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Volume 18, Issue 4, Pages (January 2017)
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Inactivation of Medial Frontal Cortex Changes Risk Preference
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Jianing Yu, David Ferster  Neuron 
Torben Ott, Simon Nikolas Jacob, Andreas Nieder  Neuron 
Nicholas J. Priebe, David Ferster  Neuron 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Natalja Gavrilov, Steffen R. Hage, Andreas Nieder  Cell Reports 
Origin and Function of Tuning Diversity in Macaque Visual Cortex
Eye Movement Preparation Modulates Neuronal Responses in Area V4 When Dissociated from Attentional Demands  Nicholas A. Steinmetz, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Guilhem Ibos, David J. Freedman  Neuron 
Dynamics of Eye-Position Signals in the Dorsal Visual System
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
The Normalization Model of Attention
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Volume 44, Issue 3, Pages (October 2004)
Jason A. Cromer, Jefferson E. Roy, Earl K. Miller  Neuron 
Volume 92, Issue 2, Pages (October 2016)
Phase Locking of Single Neuron Activity to Theta Oscillations during Working Memory in Monkey Extrastriate Visual Cortex  Han Lee, Gregory V. Simpson,
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
by Khaled Nasr, Pooja Viswanathan, and Andreas Nieder
Gregor Rainer, Earl K Miller  Neuron 
Biased Associative Representations in Parietal Cortex
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Lysann Wagener, Maria Loconsole, Helen M. Ditz, Andreas Nieder 
Volume 50, Issue 1, Pages (April 2006)
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Presentation transcript:

Coding of Cognitive Magnitude Andreas Nieder, Earl K. Miller  Neuron  Volume 37, Issue 1, Pages 149-157 (January 2003) DOI: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3

Figure 1 Comparison of the Linear-Coding and Logarithmic-Compression Hypotheses (A and B) Linear-coding hypothesis. (A) Internal representations consist of symmetric normal distributions on a linear scale that are centered on each number and become progressively wider in proportion to increasing magnitude (“scalar variability”). The ratio of the standard deviation to the mean (i.e., the coefficient of variation) is constant across a range of quantities. (B) The distributions plotted on a logarithmic scale become asymmetric with a shallower slope toward smaller numbers. (C and D) Logarithmic-compression hypotheses. (D) Quantities are represented on a power-function or logarithmically compressed scale with constant variability across different numbers. Underlying representations will be Gaussian on a log scale. The accuracy of the representations stays invariable with increasing size of a quantity. Thus, the standard deviation, not the coefficient of variation, is constant across quantities. (C) When transformed to a linear scale, the distributions are asymmetric, but now with a shallower slope toward higher numbers relative to the mean. (Amplitudes of all distributions are kept constant for simplicity.) Both models can account for numerical magnitude and distance effects. Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)

Figure 2 Behavioral Protocol and Recording Site (A) Delayed match-to-numerosity task. Fixating monkeys were cued for a given numerosity by a sample display. The subjects had to memorize the numerosity in a 1 s delay period and match it to a subsequent test stimulus (either the first or the second test stimulus was correct in 50% of the cases) by releasing a lever. During single-unit recording, the nonmatch stimuli were one numerosity up or down. For the behavioral tests (shown here), however, a larger range of nonmatch numerosities was shown. As an example, the figure displays the protocol for sample numerosity “five.” (B) Area of recording sites (blue) in both monkeys. Mainly neurons from the ventral bank of the prefrontal cortex were recorded. Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)

Figure 3 Quantification of Behavioral Performance Curves The behavioral performance for both monkeys indicated whether they judged the first test stimulus (after the delay) as containing the same number of items as the sample display (“% same as sample”). Colors represent performance curves for a given sample numerosity. Behavioral filter functions are plotted on linear (A) and logarithmic (B) scales. The functions are asymmetric when plotted on a linear scale (note the shallower slope toward higher numerosities) (A), but are symmetric when plotted on the nonlinear logarithmic scale (B). (C and D) Individual distributions for all tested numerosities for both monkeys. Columns to the left in (C) and (D) show data plotted on a linear scale, contrasted by the same functions plotted on a logarithmic scale. To evaluate the symmetry of the behavioral and filter functions in the monkeys, a normal distribution (Gaussian, indicated by dotted lines) was fitted to the measured data, and the goodness-of-fit was derived as quantitative measure (error bars, ±SEM). Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)

Figure 4 Quantification of Gauss Fits to the Behavioral Data (A) Goodness-of-fit of Gauss functions fitted to the performance curves plotted on different scales. The goodness-of-fit was significantly better for the three nonlinear scaling schemes (error bars, ±SEM). (B) The standard deviation (sigma) of the Gauss fits for nonlinear scaling plotted against the center of the Gauss function (which is identical to the numerosity of the match stimulus). Dotted lines indicate linear fits. (The values of sigma are related to the specific compression scheme.) Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)

Figure 5 Neural Representation of Numerosities in the PFC during the Sample Period (A) The neural filter functions are asymmetric on a linear scale (note the shallower slope toward higher values for preferred numerosity “2,” for example). (B) Logarithmic transformation of the filter functions results in more symmetric distributions. (C) Goodness-of-fit for the four different scaling schemes. (D) Standard deviation values for the scaling schemes across preferred quantities (error bars, ±SEM). Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)

Figure 6 Neural Representation of Numerosities in the PFC during the Delay Period (A) Again, the neural filter functions are asymmetric on a linear scale. (B) Logarithmic transformation of the filter functions results in more symmetric distributions. (C) Goodness-of-fit for the four different scaling schemes. (D) Standard deviation values for the scaling schemes across preferred quantities (error bars, ±SEM). Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)

Figure 7 Comparison of Behavioral and Neuronal Data Weber fractions and asymmetry indices for the behavioral performance (A) and the neuronal data during sample presentation (B) and delay period (C). (D) The bandwidths between the behavioral and the neural filter functions changed proportionally and exhibited a fixed ratio of 1.5. Neuron 2003 37, 149-157DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01144-3)