Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
A Sensorimotor Role for Traveling Waves in Primate Visual Cortex
Advertisements

Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1
Joshua J. Foster, Emma M. Bsales, Russell J. Jaffe, Edward Awh 
Evidence that the Lunar Cycle Influences Human Sleep
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Volume 27, Issue 7, Pages (April 2017)
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Volume 51, Issue 6, Pages (September 2006)
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Visual Search and Attention
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Activity in Posterior Parietal Cortex Is Correlated with the Relative Subjective Desirability of Action  Michael C. Dorris, Paul W. Glimcher  Neuron 
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Takashi Sato, Aditya Murthy, Kirk G. Thompson, Jeffrey D. Schall 
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Jeremy B. Wilmer, Ken Nakayama  Neuron 
Feature-based attention in visual cortex
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Hyoung F. Kim, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex
Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Hyoung F. Kim, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
MT Neurons Combine Visual Motion with a Smooth Eye Movement Signal to Code Depth-Sign from Motion Parallax  Jacob W. Nadler, Mark Nawrot, Dora E. Angelaki,
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Volume 17, Issue 1, Pages (January 2007)
Gaze and the Control of Foot Placement When Walking in Natural Terrain
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Color Constancy for an Unseen Surface
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1
Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology 
Neurophysiology of the BOLD fMRI Signal in Awake Monkeys
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Prefrontal Control of Visual Distraction
Presentation transcript:

Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan  Current Biology  Volume 23, Issue 17, Pages 1663-1669 (September 2013) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056 Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Task and Experiment Conditions (A) Task sequence during the free exploration period. This sequence is an example of one trial. There were 30 trials per block. Each trial was comprised of four phases: object following against a gray background (i), centering of gaze (ii), searching for the target object presented in the object following phase with an array of nontarget distracters (iii), and object following amidst a cluttered background (iv). (B) An example of object following during the explicit training period. A small white cross (∼0.7° in height) appeared at each subject’s estimated PRL. Subjects were instructed to use the cross as a gaze reference point for fixation and saccadic eye movements. (C) Task sequence of the invisible scotoma experiment. The first phase was identical to the first object following phase of the main experiment except that the color and luminance of the simulated scotoma was matched to those of the background. As a result, the simulated central scotoma was invisible to the subjects. Then, subjects searched for the target object among an array of distracters presented against a uniform gray background. (For ease of visibility in the figure, target objects and the gaze reference cross were rendered at 2.5× the size used in the experiment relative to the rest of the displayed elements). Current Biology 2013 23, 1663-1669DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Development and Retention of Preferred Retinal Locus for Fixation (A) Probability density maps of the retinal positions of a target object at fixation are shown for three of the six subjects. (Refer to Table S1 for the specific number of blocks each subject performed in the different stages of the experiment.) From the top to bottom, the rows represent densities estimated from the first and last blocks of the free exploration period, density estimated from the last block of the explicit training period, density estimated from all five to seven blocks of the retention assessment, and density estimated from all five blocks when the scotoma was made invisible. The density of the fovea viewing control experiment, averaged across all control subjects, is also shown. Data from the object following components with blank background and cluttered background were combined. Each polar plot represents the visual field. The gray patch depicts the central scotoma. The red dot marks the location of peak density, which we took as the estimated location of the fixational PRL. All six subjects exhibited a rapid and spontaneous emergence of fixational PRLs near the border of the scotoma within 3 hr (eight to nine blocks). All subjects were able to further refine the precision of their fixational PRLs during a subsequent explicit training period that lasted at least 15 hr (40 blocks). Not only was the PRL retained after being established, but the same PRL was also used when the scotoma was made invisible. (B) Variance of the fixational PRL as a function of block number. Variance was defined as the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) that encompassed 68% of fixations around the mean. The blue solid line indicates the average BCEA across all five subjects who spontaneously developed a single PRL (one subject, shown here as S5, developed two PRLs); the shaded blue area indicates ±1 SD of the BCEA, showing intersubject variability. The gray rectangular box represents the distribution of BCEA from the control subjects in the fovea viewing condition (±1 SD). A gap of five blocks was inserted on the abscissa between free exploration and explicit training to denote a break of 1 to 2 days from the experiment between these two stages. The BCEA of the fixation PRLs from the five subjects with a single PRL decreased rapidly within a short period of time without explicit training. Explicit training reduced the BCEA to the range of foveal fixation. It also eliminated one of the two PRLs of S5, as demonstrated in the retention test. Current Biology 2013 23, 1663-1669DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Development and Retention of Rereferenced Saccades Measured by the First Saccade Landing Site (A) Probability density maps of the retinal position of a target object at the completion of the first saccade after target movement (same format as in Figure 2A). (B) Variance (BCEA) of the first saccade landing site as a function of block number (same format as in Figure 2B). The first saccade landing site after each target movement was near the fixational PRL and distant from the fovea. Similar to the fixational PRL, the variance of the first saccade landing sites decreased rapidly within a short period of time without explicit training, demonstrating a spontaneous shift in oculomotor reference from the fovea to the PRL. With explicit training, the BCEA was further reduced to the normal range of intact foveal vision. Current Biology 2013 23, 1663-1669DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Changes in Eye Movement Characteristics during Object Following (A) Median saccade latency (the time taken to make the first saccade upon object movement) as a function of block number (same format as in Figure 2B). (B) Mean number of saccades per object movement (i.e., the number of corrective saccades plus one) as a function of block number. (C) Mean duration of fixation between object movement as a function of block number. Error bars represent ±1 SD. Current Biology 2013 23, 1663-1669DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Changes in Visual Search Performance (A) Visual search accuracy (percent correct) as a function of block number (same format as in Figure 2B). The mean accuracy level (89% ± 2.1% SD) was similar to that of the control group and was constant through the course of the experiment. (B) Time taken to complete a search trial (either target present or target absent) as a function of block number. (C) Number of saccades required for the completion of a search trial. Only correct trials were included for (B) and (C). Error bars represent ±1 SD. Current Biology 2013 23, 1663-1669DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions