Figure Legend: From: The absolute threshold of cone vision

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2015;15(13):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Davida Teller Award Lecture 2013: The importance of prediction and anticipation in the control of smooth pursuit eye movements Journal of Vision.
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(11):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(11):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(6):2. doi: /9.6.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(8):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Salience of unique hues and implications for color theory
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(6):10. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
From: Occlusion-related lateral connections stabilize kinetic depth stimuli through perceptual coupling Journal of Vision. 2009;9(10):20. doi: /
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Subjectively homogeneous noise over written text as a tool to investigate the perceptual mechanisms involved in reading Journal of Vision. 2013;13(11):14.
From: Hue shifts produced by temporal asymmetries in chromatic signals depend on the alignment of the first and second harmonics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):3.
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
Figure Legend: From: The natural statistics of blur
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
From: Nonlinearities in color coding: Compensating color appearance for the eye's spectral sensitivity Journal of Vision. 2006;6(9):12. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: The color lexicon of the Somali language
From: Crowding is tuned for perceived (not physical) location
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
From: Does spatial invariance result from insensitivity to change?
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
From: Low-level awareness accompanies “unconscious” high-level processing during continuous flash suppression Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):3. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
Figure Legend: statistical inference
From: Perceptual entrainment of individually unambiguous motions
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(6):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A new analytical method for characterizing nonlinear visual processes with stimuli of arbitrary distribution: Theory and applications Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(7):5. doi: /3.7.5 Figure Legend:
From: Linear–nonlinear models of the red–green chromatic pathway
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The psychometric function: The lapse rate revisited
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e3 (December 2017)
Presentation transcript:

Figure Legend: From: The absolute threshold of cone vision Journal of Vision. 2011;11(1):21. doi:10.1167/11.1.21 Figure Legend: Signal detection theory (SDT; Green & Swets, 1966) predicts that in distinguishing a signal (gray distribution) from noise (blue distribution) observers can increase sensitivity and lower detection thresholds by using more lenient response criteria (represented by colored vertical lines and points, ranging from pink to red, and numbered 1 (strictest) to 6 (most lenient)). (A) Low, medium, and high intensity stimuli (left, center, and right; row 1, also indicated by the dashed vertical gray lines in row 2) are detected against irreducible noise. Detection thresholds are interpolated at 50% seen (at the intersection of the solid gray line and the frequency of seeing curves, row 2, center) after adjusting for the nonzero false positive rate as in Equation 1. Detection thresholds are still seen to decrease after this adjustment (row 2, right) indicating a net gain in performance with more lenient criteria. Row 2 right shows the expected Poisson noise-limited reduction in detection threshold (solid curve). (B) An intrinsic barrier limits access to signal information (gray boxes, row 1). Subjects cannot reduce the detection criterion below this point. They can continue to increase the false positive rate by guessing, but the rates of seeing and of false positives increase in step and there is no further improvement in threshold (row 2). Row 2 (right) shows limited threshold reduction (plateau, dashed line) in the face of an intrinsic barrier to information access. All distributions shown are Poisson probability density functions. Date of download: 1/1/2018 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2018. All rights reserved.