Week 12. NP movement Text 9.2 & 9.3 English Syntax.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
BBN-ANG-253 Advanced Syntax Lecture Course Autumn, 2014/15
Advertisements

Lexical Functional Grammar History: –Joan Bresnan (linguist, MIT and Stanford) –Ron Kaplan (computational psycholinguist, Xerox PARC) –Around 1978.
NP Movement Passives, Raising: When NPs are not in their theta positions.
Week 9a. A-movement (and a bit more head-movement)
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 2 Introduction to Linguistic Theory, Part 4.
Grammatical Relations and Lexical Functional Grammar Grammar Formalisms Spring Term 2004.
Week 12b. Relative clauses CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Relative clauses Another place where we see wh- movement, besides in explicit questions (either in the.
Week 10a. VP-internal subjects and ECM CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 14. Finishing up from last time and some commentary… CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Head to Head Movement Deriving word orders that X-bar theory can’t account for.
Week 8b. Head-movement CAS LX 522 Syntax I. The puzzle so far. Head-order and specifier-order parameters can derive the some but not all types of language:
Episode 4b. UTAH CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Where we are We’ve just come up with an analysis of sentences with ditransitive verbs, such as Pat gave.
Week 11. Interim summary and some things to do in class. CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Episode 7b. Subjects, agreement, and case CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 4. q Theory and the Big Picture
Installment 10b. Raising, etc CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 9b. A-movement cont’d
Episode 8a. Passives and remaining issues CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 8. Control and PRO CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Some mid-term policy decisions and clarifications Proper names in English as DPs with Ø D. Full clauses are.
Week 8. Midterm debrief CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Midterm results Mean: 88 Mean: 88 Median: 93 Median: 93 A A- B+ B B-
Week 6. NP/DP movement and Case
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 5. Head movement.
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 8. Control and PRO.
1 Introduction to Computational Linguistics Eleni Miltsakaki AUTH Fall 2005-Lecture 2.
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 10b. VP shells.
Week 13a. QR CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Quantifiers We interpret Bill saw everyone as We interpret Bill saw everyone as For every person x, Bill saw x. For.
Week 5. Head movement CAS LX 522 Syntax I. X-bar parameters Many (most?) languages of the world have something like a basic word order, an order in which.
Episode 7b. Subjects, agreement, and case CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Syntax Lecture 3: The Subject. The Basic Structure of the Clause Recall that our theory of structure says that all structures follow this pattern: It.
Episode 8a. Passives and remaining issues CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 14b. PRO and control CAS LX 522 Syntax I. It is likely… This satisfies the EPP in both clauses. The main clause has Mary in SpecIP. The embedded.
Week 8a. Adjunction and head-movement
Week 6a. Case and checking (with a little more  -Theory) CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 9. Wh-movement.
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 11a. Wh-movement.
Syntax Nuha AlWadaani.
Linguistic Theory Lecture 3 Movement. A brief history of movement Movements as ‘special rules’ proposed to capture facts that phrase structure rules cannot.
Embedded Clauses in TAG
1 LIN 1310B Introduction to Linguistics Prof: Nikolay Slavkov TA: Qinghua Tang CLASS 14, Feb 27, 2007.
Extending X-bar Theory DPs, TPs, and CPs. The Puzzle of Determiners  Specifier RuleXP  (YP) X’ – requires the specifier to be phrasal – *That the book.
Week 4. q Theory and the Big Picture
Installment 11b. Still more loose ends about A-movement (Chapter 8, more or less) CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Syntax Lecture 8: Verb Types 1. Introduction We have seen: – The subject starts off close to the verb, but moves to specifier of IP – The verb starts.
ENGLISH SYNTAX Introduction to Transformational Grammar.
Rules, Movement, Ambiguity
Week 14b. Bonus section: Articulating the tree CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
1 LIN 1310B Introduction to Linguistics Prof: Nikolay Slavkov TA: Qinghua Tang CLASS 16, March 6, 2007.
A movement 2 Oct. 31, 2012 – Day 26 Introduction to Syntax ANTH 3590/7590 Harry Howard Tulane University.
 Chapter 8 (Part 2) Transformations Transformational Grammar Engl 424 Hayfa Alhomaid.
Syntax Lecture 6: Missing Subjects of Non-finite Clauses.
◦ Process of describing the structure of phrases and sentences Chapter 8 - Phrases and sentences: grammar1.
Lecture 1: Trace Theory.  We have seen that things move :  Arguments move out of the VP into subject position  Wh-phrases move out of IP into CP 
Week 11. Interim summary and some things to do in class. CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
1 Some English Constructions Transformational Framework October 2, 2012 Lecture 7.
Week 9. Verb movement: Aspectual Auxiliaries English Syntax.
Lec. 10.  In this section we explain which constituents of a sentence are minimally required, and why. We first provide an informal discussion and then.
Week 3. Clauses and Trees English Syntax. Trees and constituency A sentence has a hierarchical structure Constituents can have constituents of their own.
Week 10 X-bar syntax: More on Clauses English Syntax.
Week 10 X-bar syntax: More on Clauses
Week 11. Verb movement: Aspectual Auxiliaries
English Syntax Week 12. NP movement Text 9.2 & 9.3.
Syntax Lecture 9: Verb Types 1.
Lecture 4b: Verb Processes
Lecture 8: Verb Positions
ENG 3306 Raising and Control I.
: 2018.
Week 6. NP/DP movement and Case
Principles and Parameters (I)
Syntax Lecture 12: Extended VP.
Presentation transcript:

Week 12. NP movement Text 9.2 & 9.3 English Syntax

Verb Movement: French –Jean mange souvent des pommes. –Jean eats often of.the apples –‘ Jean often eat apples. ’ –John often eats apples. If we suppose that the French sentence starts out just like the English sentence, we have the underlying DS (deep structure) representation shown here. What needs to happen to get the correct surface word order? V mange V VP des pommes PP I I IP Jean DP [PRES] V AdvP souvent DS

French –Jean mange souvent des pommes. –Jean eats often the apples –‘ Jean often eats apples. ’ Of course — the V (mange) moves up to the I position. This always happens in French with a tensed/agreeing verb. This generally doesn ’ t happen in English. Hence, the difference in “ adverb position ” (really, of course, it ’ s verb position) titi V VP des pommes PP V i +I I IP Jean NP mange+ [PRES] V AdvP souvent SS

Why does this happen? Why would a language need to move its verb up to tense? In French, verbs are marked for tense and agreement — past tense verbs look different from present tense verbs, which look different from future tense verbs. If the tense information is in I ( [PRES] ), and the verb reflects this, somehow the verb needs to get together with I. French does this by moving the verb to I. (like English aspectual auxiliaries, be and have) English does this by moving I (-ed) to the verb.

Movement Today, we’re going to look at another kind of movement, the movement of NPs. In many respects, the idea is similar—an NP will originate in one place in the DS and will appear in a different place in the SS. titi V VP PP I IP NP mange V AdvP SS T I ViVi [PRES]

It is likely… Let’s consider the sentence It is likely that Mary left. Likely has one  -role to assign (Proposition), which it assigns to its complement, the embedded CP. Consider leave in the embedded clause. Leave also has one  -role to assign, which it assigns to Mary. Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C that IP Mary left V be V VP I [pres] I IP  DS 

It is likely… Notice that both  -roles are assigned to things that are in the same clause as the predicate that assigns the  -role. This is a general property of  - role assignment: A  -role must be assigned locally (within the same clause). Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C that IP Mary left V be V VP I [pres] I IP  DS 

It is likely… Moving to SS… Because the EPP requires SpecIP to be filled, Expletive Insertion applies, inserting it into SpecIP, resulting in this SS representation. This is the story of It is likely that Mary left. Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C that IP Mary left titi V VP V i +I is I IP N NP N it SS

It is likely… Now, consider: –Mary is likely [to leave]. We already know a lot about this sentence; we know that likely has one  -role to assign, which it assigns to the embedded clause, we know that leave has one  -role to assign, which it assigns to Mary. There are two problems here: –The embedded clause has no subject (*EPP) –The  -role assigned to Mary seems to be assigned outside of its clause.

It is likely… –Mary is likely [to leave] Concerning  -roles, it’s clear from the meaning that leave really does assign its  - role to Mary and not likely (Mary is leaving). This is definitely not local—Mary is not in the same clause as leave. EPP: Extended Projection Principle 

It is likely… –Mary is likely [to leave] And with respect to the EPP, we see that although the main clause IP has something in its specifier (Mary), the embedded clause seems to have nothing. How can we reconcile this?

It is likely… –Mary is likely [to leave] For  -role assignment to be local, Mary has to be in the same clause.  -role assignment takes place at DS, after which movement rules (like verb-movement from last time) apply. We can solve both problems at once by supposing that Mary moves from the embedded subject position at DS to the main clause subject position at SS. –DS: — is likely [Mary to leave] –SS: Mary i is likely [ t i to leave]

It is likely… That is, we start out with Mary in the embedded clause, in the specifier of IP, receiving its  -role locally. IP Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C Ø NP Mary V be V VP I [pres] I DS  VP I to I IP leave

It is likely… That is, we start out with Mary in the embedded clause, in the specifier of IP, receiving its  -role locally. Then Mary moves up to SpecIP in the main clause by SS. Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C Ø NP i Mary tjtj V VP V j +I is I IP SS VP I to I IP leave titi

It is likely… Notice that this satisfies the EPP in both clauses. The main clause has Mary in SpecIP. The embedded clause has the trace in SpecIP. NP i Mary IP Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C Ø tjtj V VP V j +I is I SS VP I to I IP leave titi

It is likely… This type of movement is called NP-movement. This specific instance of NP-movement, where we move a subject from an embedded clause to a higher clause is generally called subject-to-subject raising. NP i Mary Adj likely Adj AdjP C CP C Ø tjtj V VP V j +I is I IP SS VP T to T TP leave titi

Passive Now, recall the passive. The passive form of a verb seems to directly affect the theta grid of a verb; consider: –Bill ate the sandwich. –The sandwich was eaten. Eat has two  -roles to assign. By putting it in the passive, we seem to have transitive (two  -role) verb into an intransitive (one  -role) verb.

Passive –Bill ate the sandwich. Here, Bill is the Agent (gets the  -role including Agent) and the sandwich is the Theme(Patient) (gets the  -role including Theme). –The sandwich was eaten (by Bill). In the passive, the roles are the same but now the Theme is the subject and the Agent is in an optional by-phrase (a PP).

Passive Since optional thematic relations do not get included in the  -grid, what we conclude about the passive is that it changes the  -grid of the verb by removing the external  - role. eatAgentTheme ij eat+enAgentTheme ij

Passive Now, what does the structure of a passive sentence look like? There are two possibilities we could entertain. –The Theme in the passive becomes an external  - role (as opposed to in the active, where the Theme gets an internal  -role). –The Theme in both cases gets an internal  -role, but in the passive, it moves to the subject position. Let’s pursue the second option first…

Active Let’s start with the DS tree for the active sentence, Bill ate the sandwich. Here, the (internal) Theme  -role is assigned to the object NP and the (external) Agent  -role is assigned to the subject NP. Now, suppose that for the passive we simply eliminate the external  - role… NP the sandwich V eat V VP I [past] I IP  DS  NP Bill

Passive (The passive also requires the addition of the auxiliary verb be, but this is not relevant to the point at hand) We have changed the main verb to the passive form, thereby removing the external  -role, leaving us with this DS for –The sandwich was eaten. Now, what needs to happen? DP the sandwich V eaten V VP T [past] T TP DS  V be V VP

Passive –The sandwich was eaten. Now, what needs to happen? –SpecIP must be filled (EPP). –The word order needs to be altered from was eaten the sandwich to the sandwich was eaten. It should be clear where this is going—here, we posit another instance of NP-movement, like with raising. In the passive, the object moves to SpecIP satisfying the EPP. NP the sandwich V eaten V VP I [-present] I IP DS  V be V VP

Passive –The sandwich was eaten. So, to review, the idea is that the active and the passive have very similar DS representations, except that the passive has had its external  -role removed and thus no subject is generated in SpecIP (as required by the Theta Criterion). Then the object moves into SpecIP, satisfying the EPP at SS. titi V eaten V VP V j +I was I IP SS tjtj V VP NP i the sandwich

Passive –The sandwich was eaten by Bill. As for the optionally expressed Agent in the by-phrase, we take this to be like any optionally expressed adjoined phrase, a PP adjoined to V. As expected, the by-phrase can be re-ordered with respect to other adjuncts. –The sandwich was eaten… –…by Bill under the tree at noon. –…under the tree by Bill at noon. –…at noon under the tree by Bill. V VP V j +I was I IP SS tjtj V VP NP i the sandwich titi V eaten V PP by Bill