Child Motivation, Shared Book Reading, and Vocabulary Development: A Growth Mixture Modeling Approach Christine Meng Curriculum and Instruction University.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
1 When DAP Meets GAP Promoting Peaceful Coexistence between Developmentally Appropriate Practice & the Need to Address the Achievement Gap International.
Advertisements

1 Social and emotional problems can impair early learning and competence Roughly 10% of children in kindergarten show disruptive emotional or behavioral.
Highlighting Parent Involvement in Education
DELAWARE EARLY LITERACY INITIATIVE Dr. Jim J
Introduction to the Environment Rating Scales
Judith J. Carta, Ph.D. Juniper Gardens Children’s Project University of Kansas with help from Elaine Carlson, WESTAT.
3 High expectations for every child
Home and pre-school influences on early language and reading Evidence from the Effective Pre-school, Primary and Secondary Education (EPPSE) project.
Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No: HRD Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations.
Children and Poverty McLoyd (1998) Childhood poverty is a major problem in the US –Over 22% of children in the US live in poverty as compared to 9% in.
Social inclusion of young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder in Australian early childhood programs Sue Walker and Donna Berthelsen Queensland University.
Massachusetts Early Care and Education and School Readiness Study
Abstract Investigated the mother-child relationship and adolescents’ emotional symptoms and adaptive skills reported by youth. Negative mother-child relationship.
Jessica Hernandez California State University of Long Beach School of Social Work May 2012.
TM Confidential and Proprietary. Copyright © 2007 by Educational Testing Service. 1.
1 When DAP Meets GAP Promoting Peaceful Coexistence between Developmentally Appropriate Practice & the Need to Address the Achievement Gap National Association.
The Achievement Gap: Lessons from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study – Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) Tamara Halle, Nicole Forry, Elizabeth Hair & Kate Perper.
BACKGROUND RESEARCH QUESTIONS  Does the time parents spend with children differ according to parents’ occupation?  Do occupational differences remain.
© 2008 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. 1 CHAPTER 5 Sociocultural Diversity.
Associations between Child Care Quality & Developmental Outcomes of Children in Low Income Working Families in Four Communities James Elicker, Soo-Young.
This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No: HRD Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations.
Women, Minorities, and Technology Jacquelynne Eccles (PI), Pamela Davis-Kean (co-PI), and Oksana Malanchuk University of Michigan.
Teacher-Student Relationships: A Study of Teachers’ Perceptions of Their Roles and Responsibilities and Their Relational Management Practices Elizabeth.
Educational Psychology Part III Draw and discuss the significant aspects of the model of the teaching/learning process presented in class (or discussed.
UNDERSTANDING THE CLASS
What influences English and Mathematics attainment at age 11? Evidence from the EPPSE project.
The Targeted Reading Intervention: How Early Reading Intervention for Rural Kindergarten and First-Grade Students Affects Teachers’ Ratings of Students’
March 2010 what the school readiness data mean for Harford County’s children ©
BURLINGTON-EDISON SCHOOL DISTRICT APRIL 7 TH, 2014 Highlighting Parent Involvement in Education.
Links to Positive Parenting among African American and Hispanic American Low-Income Mothers Laura D. Pittman Psychology Department Northern Illinois University.
Father Involvement and Child Well-Being: 2006 Survey of Income and Program Participation (SIPP) Child Well-Being Topical Module 1 By Jane Lawler Dye Fertility.
Growing Up In Ireland Research Conference The Education of 9-Year-Olds.
Delaware Birth to Three Early Intervention System Evaluation: Child Outcomes July 15, 2004 Conference Call Series: Measuring Child Outcomes “Examples of.
Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Adults Reading to Two Year Old Children: A Population-based Study Olivia Sappenfield Emory University School of Public Health.
The Link Between Thriving Children and Economic Security: Creating Equity in Early Childhood for Our Common Good.
Printed by Parent-Adolescent Relationship Quality and the Development of Romantic Values Jessica K. Winkles, Joseph P. Allen University.
Early Childhood Environment Rating Scale
Abecedarian Project. Problems With Prior Research few early childhood programs have been sufficiently well controlled to permit scientists to evaluate.
Maternal Romantic Relationship Quality, Parenting Stress and Child Outcomes: A Mediational Model Christine R. Keeports, Nicole J. Holmberg, & Laura D.
+ Third Party Evaluation – Interim Report Presentation for Early Childhood Advisory Council December 19, 2013.
A Picture of Young Children in the U.S. Jerry West, Ph.D. National Center for Education Statistics Institute of Education Sciences EDUCATION SUMMIT ON.
Grandmothers’ Involvement among Adolescents Growing Up in Poverty Laura D. Pittman Northern Illinois University Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting.
V v Relations between Parental Marital Status, Residential Mobility, and Children’s Academic Achievement and Self-Regulation in Kindergarten Aya Bukres,
Amanda Ollis DSP PRESENTATION.  Engagement means: the teacher is actively attempting to understand the specific needs of all her students and working.
American Educational Research Association Annual Meeting AERA San Diego, CA - April 13-17, 2009 Denise Huang Examining the Relationship between LA's BEST.
Section 1. Introduction Orientation to Virginia’s QRIS.
Educational Psychology Chapter 2 By: Angela Vaughan, Katrinka Newman, Heide Alston, & Diariece Jones.
Parental Involvement By: Mike McDaniels, Brittany Broughton, Rachel Brosted, Amy Barden, and Jessica Bailey.
INTRODUCTION Maternal and paternal depression are associated with childhood externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. Few studies have examined.
Ready At Five & Maryland State Department of Education.
Sociocultural Influences Related to Language, Cognition, and Social Emotional Developmental Relationships Presenters: Kimberly Sharkins & Dr. James Ernest.
Protective Effects of Language Development Among Children in Head Start: A Person-Centered Approach Christine Meng Curriculum and Instruction University.
Fighting Behavior among early adolescent African Americans: What are the personal and environmental factors? Vanya Jones, PhD, MPH APHA Session ,
Selfishness and Self-Mattering: Do I Fit In? Leah Burke Advisors: Ellen Cohn, Ph.D. & Alexander Blandina, M.A. The University of New Hampshire INTRODUCTION.
Dr Karuppiah Nirmala.
Copyright 2012 Pearson Education. Vukelich, Helping Young Children Learn Language and Literacy: Birth Through Kindergarten 3/e Chapter 1 Foundations of.
Introduction Results: Moderated Mediation The Immigrant Paradox: Associations Between Latino Kindergartners’ Reading and Mathematics Skills, Parents’ Knowledge.
1 Title I Annual Parent Meeting September 16, 2013.
Our Future: Kindergarten Readiness Study for the Le Sueur-Henderson School District Layne Wilbright Minnesota State University, Mankato November 15, 2014.
Math, Reading, Writing Growth and Wellbeing in Adolescents Kevin L. Gess The Citadel Reading, Writing, and Math play a big part in a young student’s life.
Using Self-Regulation Research to Inform Intervention Strategies for Latino Preschoolers to Enhance School Readiness Tatiana Nogueira Peredo a, Margaret.
Reciprocal Relations Between Parent-Child Relationship Quality and Children's Adjustment During Early Childhood Chelsea M. Weaver, Anne M. Gill, Katelyn.
Development of Physical Aggression: Exploring the Relationship with Language Elizabeth Anson MS Kimberly Sidora-Arcoleo PhD Robert Cole PhD Harriet Kitzman.
Indiana Paths to QUALITY™ Child Care Quality Rating and Improvement System: Outcomes for Children and Child Care Providers James Elicker, Zachary Gold,
Texas Pediatric Society Electronic Poster Contest
Using Relationships of Support to Nurture the Language of Emotions
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Study Design/Methods Used
Dorothy S. Strickland, Ph.D. Rutgers, The State University of NJ
Presentation transcript:

Child Motivation, Shared Book Reading, and Vocabulary Development: A Growth Mixture Modeling Approach Christine Meng Curriculum and Instruction University of Wisconsin-Madison INTRODUCTION Head Start children have been shown to have a higher rate of difficulties in literacy and language development due to poverty. The home environment, in particular shared book reading, may be related to children’s language development. While the extant research has link shared book reading with children’s vocabulary development, the nature of the association is still not clear, particularly with respect to the pathways through which shared book reading and children’s vocabulary development are linked. Prior research has identified parents’ reading beliefs (Cottone, 2012), teachers’ curriculum goals (Massetti & Bracken, 2010), and mothers’ education level (Korat, 2009) as the mechanisms that explain this association. However, what is less understood is the role of child motivation. Studies show that children who show greater persistence and more attention tend to have better academic performance (Berhenke, Miller, Brown, Seifer, & Dickstein, 2011; Skinner & Belmont, 1993). These studies demonstrate that children who are more motivated tend to pursue more learning opportunities, resulting in greater academic performance. To examine child motivation, shared book reading, and the trajectories of vocabulary development among children of low-income families, this study used a nationally representative sample of Head Start children, with the emphasis on child motivation as the moderator. Specifically, this study examined (1) direct effects of child motivation and shared book reading on the trajectories of vocabulary development, and (2) child motivation as a factor that moderated the effect of shared book reading on the trajectories of vocabulary development. RESULTS Two classes of vocabulary development emerged from the results of growth mixture modeling: a decrease-increase class and a decrease-only class. Approximately 66% of Head Start children belonged to the decrease-increase class. Approximately 34% of Head Start children belonged to the decrease-only class. The results for the first research question showed that shared book reading, competence motivation, and persistence had direct effect on the trajectories of vocabulary development. For the decrease-increase class, shared book reading, competence motivation, and persistence had positive impact on the vocabulary scores in the first kindergarten year and the vocabulary growth and acceleration over time. For the decrease-only class, competence motivation was the only factor that had positive impact on the vocabulary growth. The results for the second research question showed that the effect of shared book reading on Head Start children’s vocabulary development depended on the children’s learning behavior. Higher levels of shared book reading predicted higher levels of vocabulary growth when the children had higher levels of learning behavior. Children with higher levels of learning behavior scored higher on the vocabulary test than those with lower levels of learning behavior. In the context of shared book reading, parents may choose interesting reading materials to increase children’s interest in print, thereby increasing children’s positive attitude and confidence toward reading. This finding appears to suggest a potential bidirectional relationship between shared book reading and children’s learning behavior. Shared book reading can increase children’s interest and motivation to read, which in turn may engage in more shared book reading activities. Future research needs to consider the long-term bidirectional process between shared book reading and children’s learning behavior to understand whether shared book reading and children’s learning behavior reciprocally influence each other. DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to understand (1) the direct effect of shared book reading and children’s learning motivation on Head Start children’s vocabulary trajectories; and (2) the indirect, moderating factor of children’s learning motivation on the effect of shared book reading on Head Start children’s vocabulary trajectories. The results showed heterogeneous vocabulary trajectories in this population. Head Start children who demonstrated vocabulary growth were those who had parents who frequently read to them and showed higher levels of motivation to learn. On the other hand, Head Start children whose vocabulary skills remained low throughout the four measurement occasions were children who showed competence motivation. It appears there is a double dosage effect for the combination of shared book reading and children’s competence motivation. Higher levels of shared book reading predicted higher levels of vocabulary growth when the children had higher levels of competence motivation. Children with higher levels of competence motivation scored higher on the vocabulary test than those with lower levels of learning behavior. The moderating role of children’s learning behavior on vocabulary growth may be due to the extent to which the parents structure the home environment to provide their children literacy experience. A structured family literacy environment can facilitate children’s positive perception of their reading competence via parent involvement and autonomy support. Parents who are involved in their children’s education are likely to attend the parent-teacher conference, help their children with school homework, talk to the teacher about what books they should read to their children at home, and so forth. Similarly, parents’ autonomy support can increase young children’s vocabulary development through scaffolding to support the children’s reading competence. The reason for parents to provide support and scaffold is to help young children regulate their motivation and engagement with print. Over time the children will be able to internalize these external regulations so as to be intrinsically motivated to read. Provided that two developmental trajectories of vocabulary are identified, future research needs to examine how the interaction between child motivation and shared book reading plays out for Head Start children with different developmental trajectories. The results have implications for shared book reading interventions and educators. Educators and parents need to be aware that passively reading to their children may not be the best way to motivate the children to read. Engaging the children in the story and providing feedback to scaffold children’s reading competence may be more likely to facilitate children’s comprehension and vocabulary growth. DATASET The data used for this project came from the Head Start Family and Child Experiences Survey (FACES—2003 Cohort). The FACES is a national longitudinal study that examines the cognitive, emotional, social, and physical development of the Head Start children in the United States. The FACES study also includes variables that examine the characteristics and well-being of families, the observed quality of Head Start classrooms, and the characteristics and opinions of Head Start teachers and other program staff. The FACES included a sample of 1,778 children who were three and four years old and their parents in a stratified national probability sample of 43 Head Start programs. The sample was stratified by three variables: region of the country, urbanicity, and percentage of minority families in the program. The following Head Start programs were excluded: Migrant and Seasonal Head Start programs, American Indian/Alaska Native Head Start programs, Early Head Start programs, programs in the United States territories, and programs that do not serve children directly. REFERENCES Berhenke, A., Miller, A. L., Brown, E., Seifer, R., & Dickstein, S. (2011). Observed emotional and behavioral indicators of motivation predict school readiness in Head Start graduates. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, doi: /j.ecresq Cottone, E. A. (2012). Preschoolers’ emergent literacy skills: The mediating role of maternal reading beliefs. Early Education and Development, 23, doi: / Korat, O. (2009). The effect of maternal teaching talk on children's emergent literacy as a function of type of activity and maternal education level. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, doi: /j.appdev Massetti, G. M., & Bracken, S. S. (2010). Classroom academic and social context: Relationships among emergent literacy, behavioural functioning and teacher curriculum goals in kindergarten. Early Child Development and Care, 180, doi: / Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, doi: / Data Sources *The Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, the Administration for Children and Families, the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services *Child Care & Early Education Research Connections ABSTRACT Based on the sociocultural theory and the expectancy-value theory, this study (1) examined direct effects of shared book reading and child motivation on the vocabulary trajectories, and (2) whether child motivation moderated the effect of shared book reading on the vocabulary trajectories. The growth mixture modeling was performed to address the research questions with a nationally representative sample of Head Start children. Two latent classes of vocabulary trajectories were found. Shared book reading, competence motivation, and persistence had direct effects on vocabulary growth. Motivation emerged as a significant moderator—highly motivated children had more growth in vocabulary regardless of the levels of shared book reading. However, higher levels of shared book reading were more important for less motivated children. METHOD SAMPLE The sample included a total of 2,612 children and their parents or the primary caregivers (mean annual family income = $16,457.75, SD = 12, ). The family structure was diverse (48% two-parent household, 47% mother-only single-parent household, 2% father-only single-parent household, and 3% had neither a mother nor a father as the head of the household). The children’s mean age at program entry was four years old (49% male, 51% female; 29% Caucasian, 31% African American, 33% Hispanic, 7% others). The mothers’ mean age in fall 2003 was years old (SD = 6.24) with 67% having more than high school education. The fathers’ mean age in fall 2003 was years old (SD = 7.38) with 64% having more than high school education. The parents had diverse race and ethnicity backgrounds (mothers: 34% Caucasian, 30% African American, 32% Hispanics, and 4% others; fathers: 30% Caucasian, 34% African American, 31% Hispanics, 5% others). MEASURES Children’s vocabulary was measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III) (Dunn & Dunn, 1997). Shared book reading was measured by parental report of reading to the child in the past week on a scale of 1 (not at all in last week) to 4 (every day). Children’s competence motivation and persistence were measured by the Preschool Learning Behavior Scale (McDermott, Green, Francis, & Stott, 2000). ANALYSIS RQ1 was examined by using growth mixture modeling to determine the number of classes of vocabulary development. RQ2 was examined by using latent growth curve analysis to regress the predictors on the three latent growth factors (intercept, linear slope, and quadratic slope) while controlling for the amount of reading materials at home, child age, child gender, child disability status, family annual income, family structure, mother education, and child ethnicity.