Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

580.691 Learning Theory Reza Shadmehr State estimation theory.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "580.691 Learning Theory Reza Shadmehr State estimation theory."— Presentation transcript:

1 580.691 Learning Theory Reza Shadmehr State estimation theory

2 AB Subject was instructed to pull on a knob that was fixed on a rigid wall. A) EMG recordings from arm and leg muscles. Before biceps is activated, the brain activates the leg muscles to stabilize the lower body and prevent sway due to the anticipated pulling force on the upper body. B) When a rigid bar is placed on the upper body, the leg muscles are not activated when biceps is activated. (Cordo and Nashner, 1982)

3 AB 100 Hz Effect of eye movement on the memory of a visual stimulus. In the top panel, the filled circle represents the fixation point, the asterisk indicates the location of the visual stimulus, and the dashed circle indicates the receptive field a cell in the LIP region of the parietal cortex. A) Discharge to the onset and offset of a visual stimulus in the cell’s receptive field. Abbreviations: H. eye, horizontal eye position; Stim, stimulus; V. eye, vertical eye position. B) Discharge during the time period in which a saccade brings the stimulus into the cell’s receptive field. The cell’s discharge increased before the saccade brought the stimulus into the cell’s receptive field. (From (Duhamel et al., 1992)

4 Why predict sensory consequences of motor commands?

5 Subject looked at a moving cursor while a group of dots appeared on the screen for 300ms. In some trials the dots would remain still (A) while in other trials they would move together left or right with a constant speed (B). Subject indicated the direction of motion of the dots. From this result, the authors estimated the speed of subjective stationarity, i.e., the speed of dots for which the subject perceived them to be stationary. C) The unfilled circles represent performance of control subjects. Regardless of the speed of the cursor, they perceived the dots to be stationary only if their speed was near zero. The filled triangles represent performance of subject RW. As the speed of the cursor increased, RW perceived the dots to be stationary if their speed was near the speed of the cursor. (Haarmeier et al., 1997)

6 Disorders of agency in schizophrenia relate to an inability to compensate for sensory consequences of self-generated motor commands. In a paradigm similar to that shown in the last figure, volunteers estimated whether during motion of a cursor the background moved to the right or left. By varying the background speed, at each cursor speed the experimenters estimated the speed of perceptual stationarity, i.e., the speed of background motion for which the subject saw the background to be stationary. They then computed a compensation index as the difference between speed of eye movement and speed of background when perceived to be stationary, divided by speed of eye movement. The subset of schizophrenic patients who had delusional symptoms showed a greater deficit than control in their ability to compensate for sensory consequences of self-generated motor commands. (From (Lindner et al., 2005))

7 A B C D Combining predictions with observations

8 Parameter variance depends only on input selection and noise A noisy process produces n data points and we form an ML estimate of w. We run the noisy process again with the same sequence of x’s and re- estimate w: The distribution of the resulting w will have a var- cov that depends only on the sequence of inputs, the bases that encode those inputs, and the noise sigma.

9 The Gaussian distribution and its var-cov matrix A 1-D Gaussian distribution is defined as In n dimensions, it generalizes to When x is a vector, the variance is expressed in terms of a covariance matrix C, where ρij corresponds to the degree of correlation between variables xi and xj

10 -20123 -2 0 1 2 3 -20123 -2 0 1 2 3 x1 and x2 are positively correlatedx1 and x2 are not correlatedx1 and x2 are negatively correlated

11 Parameter uncertainty: Example 1 Input history: 100.5 10 10 10 01 x1 was “on” most of the time. I’m pretty certain about w1. However, x2 was “on” only once, so I’m uncertain about w2.

12 Parameter uncertainty: Example 2 Input history: 111 111 111 111 100.5 x1 and x2 were “on” mostly together. The weight var-cov matrix shows that what I learned is that: I do not know individual values of w1 and w2 with much certainty. x1 appeared slightly more often than x2, so I’m a little more certain about the value of w1.

13 Parameter uncertainty: Example 3 Input history: 010.5 01 01 01 111 x2 was mostly “on”. I’m pretty certain about w2, but I am very uncertain about w1. Occasionally x1 and x2 were on together, so I have some reason to believe that:

14 Effect of uncertainty on learning rate When you observe an error in trial n, the amount that you should change w should depend on how certain you are about w. The more certain you are, the less you should be influenced by the error. The less certain you are, the more you should “pay attention” to the error. mx1 Kalman gain error Rudolph E. Kalman (1960) A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. Transactions of the ASME–Journal of Basic Engineering, 82 (Series D): 35-45. Research Institute for Advanced Study 7212 Bellona Ave, Baltimore, MD

15 Example of the Kalman gain: running estimate of average Kalman gain: learning rate decreases as the number of samples increase As n increases, we trust our past estimate w(n-1) a lot more than the new observation y(n) Past estimateNew measure w(n) is the online estimate of the mean of y

16 Example of the Kalman gain: running estimate of variance sigma_hat is the online estimate of the var of y

17 We note that P is simply the var-cov matrix of our model weights. It represents the uncertainty in our estimates of the model parameters. We want to update the weights in such a way as to minimize the trace of this variance. The trace is the sum of the squared errors between our estimates of w and the true estimates. Some observations about variance of model parameters

18 Trace of parameter var-cov matrix is the sum of squared parameter errors Our objective is to find learning rate k (Kalman gain) such that we minimize the sum of the squared error in our parameter estimates. This sum is the trace of the P matrix. Therefore, given observation y(n), we want to find k such that we minimize the variance of our estimate w.

19 Objective: adjust learning gain in order to minimize model uncertainty a prior variance of parameters a posterior variance of parameters my estimate of w* before I see y in trial n, given that I have seen y up to n-1 error in trial n my estimate after I see y in trial n Hypothesis about data observation in trial n

20 Evolution of parameter uncertainty

21 Find K to minimize trace of uncertainty

22 scalar Find K to minimize trace of uncertainty

23 The Kalman gain If I have a lot of uncertainty about my model, P is large compared to sigma. I will learn a lot from the current error. If I am pretty certain about my model, P is small compared to sigma. I will tend to ignore the current error.

24 Update of model uncertainty Model uncertainty decreases with every data point that you observe.

25 In this model, we hypothesize that the hidden variables, i.e., the “true” weights, do not change from trial to trial. Observed variables Hidden variable A priori estimate of mean and variance of the hidden variable before I observe the first data point Update of the estimate of the hidden variable after I observed the data point Forward projection of the estimate to the next trial

26 In this model, we hypothesize that the hidden variables change from trial to trial. A priori estimate of mean and variance of the hidden variable before I observe the first data point Update of the estimate of the hidden variable after I observed the data point Forward projection of the estimate to the next trial

27 Learning rate is proportional to the ratio between two uncertainties: my model vs. my measurement. After we observe an input x, the uncertainty associated with the weight of that input decreases. Because of state update noise Q, uncertainty increases as we form the prior for the next trial. Uncertainty about my model parameters Uncertainty about my measurement

28 2. Bayesian state estimation 3. Causal Inference 4. The influence of priors 5. Behaviors that are not Bayesian

29 Comparison of Kalman gain to LMS See derivation of this in homework In the Kalman gain approach, the P matrix depends on the history of all previous and current inputs. In LMS, the learning rate is simply a constant that does not depend on past history. With the Kalman gain, our estimate converges on a single pass over the data set. In LMS, we don’t estimate the var-cov matrix P on each trial, but we will need multiple passes before our estimate converges.

30 246810 0.65 0.7 0.75 0.8 High noise in the state update model produces increased uncertainty in model parameters. This produces high learning rates. High noise in the measurement also increases parameter uncertainty. But this increase is small relative to measurement uncertainty. Higher measurement noise leads to lower learning rates. Effect of state and measurement noise on the Kalman gain

31 Learning rate is higher in a state model that has high auto- correlations (larger a). That is, if the learner assumes that the world is changing slowly (a is close to 1), then the learner will have a large learning rate. 246810 1 2 3 4 5 Effect of state transition auto-correlation on the Kalman gain

32 Kalman filter as a model of animal learning Suppose that x represents inputs from the environment: a light and a tone. Suppose that y represents rewards, like a food pellet. Animal’s model of the experimental setupAnimal’s expectation on trial n Animal’s learning from trial n

33 Various forms of classical conditioning in animal psychology Table from Peter Dayan Not explained by LMS, but predicted by the Kalman filter.

34 Sharing Paradigm Train: {x1,x2} -> 1 Test: x1 -> ?, x2 -> ? Result: x1->0.5, x2->0.5 10203040 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 010203040 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 w1 w2 10203040 0.2 0.25 0.3 0.35 0.4 0.45 0.5 P11 P22 10203040 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3 0.35 0.4 k1 k2 010203040 0 1 x1 x2 y * 10203040 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 010203040 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 w1 w2 Learning with Kalman gainLMS

35 Blocking Kamin (1968) Attention-like processes in classical conditioning. In: Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior: aversive stimulation (ed. MR Jones), pp. 9- 33. Univ. of Miami Press. Kamin trained an animal to continuously press a lever to receive food. He then paired a light (conditioned stimulus) and a mild electric shock to the foot of the rat (unconditioned stimulus). In response to the shock, the animal would reduce the lever-press activity. Soon the animal learned that the light predicted the shock, and therefore reduced lever pressing in response to the light. He then paired the light with a tone when giving the electric shock. After this second stage of training, he observed than when the tone was given alone, the animal did not reduce its lever pressing. The animal had not learned anything about the tone.

36 Blocking Paradigm Train: x1 -> 1, {x1,x2} -> 1 Test: x2 -> ?, x1 -> ? Result: x2 -> 0, x1 -> 1 010203040 0 1 x1 x2 y * 10203040 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 010203040 -0.25 0 0.25 0.5 0.75 1 1.25 w1 w2 10203040 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 P11 P22 10203040 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 k1 k2 Learning with Kalman gainLMS 010203040 -0.2 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 w1 w2 10203040 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat

37 Backwards Blocking Paradigm Train: {x1,x2} -> 1, x1 -> 1 Test: x2 -> ? Result: x2 -> 0 0102030405060 0 1 x1 x2 y * 0102030405060 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 0102030405060 -0.2 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 w1 w2 0102030405060 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 P11 P22 0102030405060 -0.4 -0.2 0 0.2 0.4 k1 k2 Learning with Kalman gainLMS 0102030405060 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 0102030405060 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 w1 w2

38 Different output models Case 1: the animal assumes an additive model. If each stimulus predicts one reward, then if the two are present together, they predict two rewards. Suppose that x represents inputs from the environment: a light and a tone. Suppose that y represents a reward, like a food pellet. Case 2: the animal assumes a weighted average model. If each stimulus predicts one reward, then if the two are present together, they still predict one reward, but with higher confidence. The weights b1 and b2 should be set to the inverse of the variance (uncertainty) with which each stimulus x1 and x2 predicts the reward.

39 General case of the Kalman filter A priori estimate of mean and variance of the hidden variable before I observe the first data point Update of the estimate of the hidden variable after I observed the data point Forward projection of the estimate to the next trial nx1 mx1

40 How to set the initial var-cov matrix In homework, we will show that in general: Now if we have absolutely no prior information on w, then before we see the first data point P(1|0) is infinity, and therefore its inverse in zero. After we see the first data point, we will be using the above equation to update our estimate. The updated estimate will become: A reasonable and conservative estimate of the initial value of P would be to set it to the above value. That is, set:

41 Data fusion Suppose that we have two sensors that independently measure something. We would like to combine their measures to form a better estimate. What should the weights be? Suppose that we know that sensor 1 gives us measurement y1 and has Gaussian noise with variance: And similarly, sensor 2 has gives us measurement y2 and has Gaussian noise with variance: A good idea is to weight each sensor inversely proportional to its noise:

42 To see why this makes sense, let’s put forth a generative model that describes our hypothesis about how the data that we are observing is generated: Observed variables Hidden variable Data fusion via Kalman filter

43 See homework for this priors our first observation variance of our posterior estimate

44 Notice that after we make our first observation, the variance of our posterior is better than the variance of either sensor. What our sensors tell us The real world

45 -20246810 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 -2.502.557.51012.515 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Sensor 1Sensor 2 Combined Sensor 1 Sensor 2 Combined Combining equally noisy sensorsCombining sensors with unequal noise Mean of the posterior, and its variance probability

46 Puzzling results: Savings and memory despite “washout” Gain=eye displacement divided by target displacement Result 1: After changes in gain, monkeys exhibit recall despite behavioral evidence for washout. Kojima et al. (2004) Memory of learning facilitates saccade adaptation in the monkey. J Neurosci 24:7531. 1

47 Result 2: Following changes in gain and a long period of washout, monkeys exhibit no recall. Result 3: Following changes in gain and a period of darkness, monkeys exhibit a “jump” in memory. Puzzling results: Improvements in performance without error feedback Kojima et al. (2004) J Neurosci 24:7531.

48 The learner’s hypothesis about the structure of the world A 1. The world has many hidden states. What I observe is a linear combination of these states. 2.The hidden states change from trial to trial. Some change slowly, others change fast. 3.The states that change fast have larger noise than states that change slow. slow system fast system state transition equation output equation

49 050100150200250300 -1.5 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 050100150200250300 0 1 050100150200250300 -0.4 -0.2 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 w1 w2 Simulations for savings x1 x2 y * The critical assumption is that in the fast system, there is much more noise than in the slow system. This produces larger learning rate in the fast system.

50 050100150200250300 -0.5 0 0.5 1 x1 x2 y * 050100150200250300 -1.5 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 050100150200250300 -0.4 -0.2 0 0.2 0.4 w1 w2 Simulations for spontaneous recovery despite zero error feedback error clamp In the error clamp period, estimates are made yet the weight update equation does not see any error. Therefore, the effect of Kalman gain in the error- clamp period is zero. Nevertheless, weights continue to change because of the state update equations. The fast weights rapidly rebound to zero, while the slow weights slowly decline. The sum of these two changes produces a “spontaneous recovery” after washout.

51 Mean gain at start of recovery = 0.83 Mean gain at start of recovery = 0.86 Mean gain at end of recovery = 0.87 % gain change = 1.2% gain change = 14.4% Mean gain at end of recovery = 0.95 Target extinguished during recoveryTarget visible during recovery Changes in representation without error feedback Seeberger et al. (2002) Brain Research 956:374-379.

52 Massed vs. Spaced training: effect of changing the inter-trial interval Learning reaching in a force field ITI = 8min ITI = 1min Discrimination performance (sec) Rats were trained on an operant conditional discrimination in which an ambiguous stimulus (X) indicated both the occasions on which responding in the presence of a second cue (A) would be reinforced and the occasions on which responding in the presence of a third cue (B) would not be reinforced (X --> A+, A-, X --> B-, B+). Both rats with lesions of the hippocampus and control rats learned this discrimination more rapidly when the training trials were widely spaced (intertrial interval of 8 min) than when they were massed (intertrial interval of 1 min). With spaced practice, lesioned and control rats learned this discrimination equally well. But when the training trials were massed, lesioned rats learned more rapidly than controls. Han, J.S., Gallagher, M. & Holland, P. Hippocampus 8:138-46 (1998)

53 Performance in a water maze Sisti, Glass, Shors (2007) Neurogenesis and the spacing effect: learning over time enhances memory and the survival of new neurons. Learning and Memory 14:368. 4 trials a day for 4 days 16 trials in one day Massed vs. Spaced training: effect of changing the inter-trial interval

54 A A AAA The learner’s hypothesis about the structure of the world 1. The world has many hidden states. What I observe is a linear combination of these states. 2.The hidden states change from trial to trial. Some change slowly, others change fast. 3.The states that change fast have larger noise than states that change slow. 4.The state changes can occur more frequently than I can make observations. Inter-trial interval

55 050100150200250300 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat 050010001500200025003000 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhat ITI=2 ITI=20

56 When there is an observation, the uncertainty for each hidden variable decreases proportional to its Kalman gain. When there are no observations, the uncertainty decreases in proportion to A squared, but increases in proportion to state noise Q. 100010201040106010801100 0.106 0.108 0.11 0.112 0.114 0.116 0.118 P22 100010201040106010801100 0.0126 0.0128 0.013 0.0132 0.0134 0.0136 P11 Uncertainty for the slow stateUncertainty for the fast state ITI=20 Beyond a minimum ITI, increased ITI continues to increase the uncertainty of the slow state but has little effect on the fast state uncertainty. The longer ITI increases the total learning by increasing the slow state’s sensitivity to error.

57 020406080100120140 0 0.5 1 1.5 y yhatspaced yhatmassed 020406080100120140 -0.2 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 w1massed w1spaced w2massed w2spaced 020406080100120140 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 k1massed k1spaced k2massed k2spaced Observation number Performance in spaced training depends largely on the slow state. Therefore, spaced training produces memories that decay little with passage of time.

58 ITI=14 ITI=2 ITI=98 Performance during training Test at 1 week ITI=14 ITI=2 ITI=98 Testing at 1 day or 1 week (averaged together) Pavlik, P. I. and Anderson, J. R. ( 2005). Practice and forgetting effects on vocabulary memory: An activation-based model of the spacing effect. Cognitive Science, 29, 559-586. Spaced training results in better retention in learning a second language On Day 1, subjects learned to translate written Japanese words into English. They were given a Japanese word (written phonetically), and then given the English translation. This “study trial” was repeated twice. Afterwards, the were given the Japanese word and had to write the translation. If their translation was incorrect, the correct translation was given. The ITI between word repetition was either 2, 14, or 98 trials. Performance during training was better when the ITI was short. However, retention was much better for words that were observed with longer ITI. (The retention test involved two groups; one at 1 day and other at 7 days. Performance was slightly better for the 1 day group but the results were averaged in this figure.)

59 DM Wolpert et al. (1995) Science 269:1880 Motor command Sensory measurement State of our body Application of Kalman filter to problems in sensorimotor control

60 When we move our arm in darkness, we may estimate the position of our hand based on three sources of information: proprioceptive feedback. a forward model of how the motor commands have moved our arm. by combining our prediction from the forward model with actual proprioceptive feedback. Experimental procedures: Subject holds a robotic arm in total darkness. The hand is briefly illuminated. An arrow is displayed to left or right, showing which way to move the hand. In some cases, the robot produces a constant force that assists or resists the movement. The subject slowly moves the hand until a tone is sounded. They use the other hand to move a mouse cursor to show where they think their hand is located. DM Wolpert et al. (1995) Science 269:1880

61 assistive resistive

62 Motor command Sensory measurement State of the body A B C The generative model, describing actual dynamics of the limb The model for estimation of sensory state from sensory feedback For whatever reason, the brain has an incorrect model of the arm. It overestimates the effect of motor commands on changes in limb position.

63 Initial conditions: the subject can see the hand and has no uncertainty regarding its position and velocity Forward model of state change and feedback Actual observation Estimate of state incorporates the prior and the observation Forward model to establish the prior and the uncertainty for the next state

64 00.20.40.60.811.21.4 0 5 10 15 20 00.20.40.60.811.21.4 Timesec 0 0.002 0.004 0.006 0.008 0.01 0.012 0.014 Actual and estimated position Kalman gain Bias at end of movement (cm) Variance at end of movement (cm^2) Total movement time (sec) 00.20.40.60.811.21.4 -1.5 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 Motor command u Time of “beep” For movements of various length A single movement Pos (cm)


Download ppt "580.691 Learning Theory Reza Shadmehr State estimation theory."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google