Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

fMRI Analysis with emphasis on the General Linear Model

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "fMRI Analysis with emphasis on the General Linear Model"— Presentation transcript:

1 fMRI Analysis with emphasis on the General Linear Model
Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute Department of Psychology Western University fMRI Analysis with emphasis on the General Linear Model Last Update: February 11, 2013 Last Course: Psychology 9223, W2013, Western University

2 Statistical Foundations

3 What data do we start with
B Each voxel is a “big box of neurons” 30 slices x 64 voxels x 64 voxels of (3 mm)3 =122,880 voxels Each voxel has a time course

4 What data do we start with
B Measured BOLD Signal We know the paradigm Mother Nature’s Convolution + Neural Activation in Response to Stimulus/Task Vasculature Error

5 What data do we start with
We know the paradigm and predicted neural activity We can model the HRF and assume it’s linear(ish) Thus we can predict an expected time course

6 We could even derive subject-specific HRFs
Choice of HRFs 20 Ss’ HRFs Handwerker et al., 2004, NI We could even derive subject-specific HRFs Two-Gamma (preferred) Boynton

7 What data do we start with
Now we can see how closely our predicted time course matches real voxel time course We know the paradigm and predicted neural activity We can model the HRF and assume it’s linear(ish) Thus we can predict an expected time course

8 A Simple Correlation Will Do
r (df =135) = 0.528 p < Amplitude of Data Time Point in 1 voxel Each dot is one time point in our subject’s data (except that I got too bored to draw all 136 time points for our run) Now we just have to repeat this 122,879 more times! Amplitude of Predictor Time Point

9 Statistical Approaches in a Nutshell
t-tests compare activation levels between two conditions use a time-shift to account for hemodynamic lag correlations model activation and see whether any areas show a similar pattern Fourier analysis Do a Fourier analysis to see if there is energy at your paradigm frequency Fourier analysis images from Huettel, Song & McCarthy, 2004, Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

10 Effect of Thresholds r = .40 16% of variance p < .000001 r = .80

11 The General Linear Model (GLM)
GLM definition from Huettel et al.: a class of statistical tests that assume that the experimental data are composed of the linear combination of different model factors, along with uncorrelated noise Model statistical model Linear things add up sensibly (1+1 = 2) note that linearity refers to the predictors in the model and not necessarily the BOLD signal General many simpler statistical procedures such as correlations, t-tests and ANOVAs are subsumed by the GLM

12 Benefits of the GLM GLM is an overarching tool that can do anything that the simpler tests do allows any combination of contrasts (e.g., intact - scrambled, scrambled - baseline), unlike simpler methods (correlations, t-tests, Fourier analyses) allows more complex designs (e.g., factorial designs) allows much greater flexibility for combining data within subjects and between subjects allows comparisons between groups allows counterbalancing orders within and between subjects allows modelling of known sources of noise in the data (e.g., error trials, head motion)

13 Composition of a Voxel Time Course

14 A Simple Experiment TIME Blank Screen Intact Objects Scrambled Objects
Lateral Occipital Complex responds when subject views objects Blank Screen Intact Objects Scrambled Objects TIME One volume (12 slices) every 2 seconds for 272 seconds (4 minutes, 32 seconds) Condition changes every 16 seconds (8 volumes)

15 What’s real? A. C. B. D.

16 = + What’s real? signal noise
I created each of those time courses based by taking the predictor function and adding a variable amount of random noise signal = + noise

17 What’s real? Which of the data sets below is more convincing?

18 Formal Statistics Formal statistics are just doing what your eyeball test of significance did Estimate how likely it is that the signal is real given how noisy the data is confidence: how likely is it that the results could occur purely due to chance? “p value” = probability value If “p = .03”, that means there is a .03/1 or 3% chance that the results are bogus By convention, if the probability that a result could be due to chance is less than 5% (p < .05), we say that result is statistically significant Significance depends on signal (differences between conditions) noise (other variability) sample size (more time points are more convincing)

19 Let’s create a time course for one LO voxel

20 We’ll begin with activation
Response to Intact Objects is 4X greater than Scrambled Objects

21 Then we’ll assume that our modelled activation is off because a transient component

22 Our modelled activation could be off for other reasons
All of the following could lead to inaccurate models different shape of function different width of function different latency of function

23 Reminder: Variability of HRF
Intersubject variability of HRF in M1 Handwerker et al., 2004, NeuroImage

24 Now let’s add some variability due to head motion

25 …though really motion is more complex
Head motion can be quantified with 6 parameters given in any motion correction algorithm x translation y translation z translation xy rotation xz rotation yz rotation For simplicity, I’ve only included parameter one in our model Head motion can lead to other problems not predictable by these parameters

26 Now let’s throw in a pinch of linear drift
linear drift could arise from magnet noise (e.g., parts warm up) or physiological noise (e.g., subject’s head sinks)

27 and then we’ll add a dash of low frequency noise
low frequency noise can arise from magnet noise or physiological noise (e.g., subject’s cycles of alertness/drowsiness) low frequency noise would occur over a range of frequencies but for simplicity, I’ve only included one frequency (1 cycle per run) here Linear drift is really just very low frequency noise

28 and our last ingredient… some high frequency noise
high frequency noise can arise from magnet noise or physiological noise (e.g., subject’s breathing rate and heartrate)

29 When we add these all together, we get a realistic time course

30 General Linear Model

31 Now let’s be the experimenter
First, we take our time course and normalize it using z scores z = (x - mean)/SD normalization leads to data where mean = zero SD = 1 Alternative: You can transform the data into % BOLD signal change. This is usually a better approach because it’s not dependent on variance

32 Wake Up!!!!! If you only pay attention to one slide in this lecture, it should be the next one!!!

33 We create a GLM with 2 predictors
× 1 = + + × 2 = fMRI Signal Design Matrix x Betas + Residuals “what we CAN explain” “how much of it we CAN explain” “what we CANNOT explain” = “our data” x + Statistical significance is basically a ratio of explained to unexplained variance

34 Implementation of GLM in SPM
Intact Predictor Scrambled Predictor Many thanks to Øystein Bech Gadmar for creating this figure in SPM  Time SPM represents time as going down SPM represents predictors within the design matrix as grayscale plots (where black = low, white = high) over time GLM includes a constant to take care of the average activation level throughout each run SPM shows this explicity (BV may not)

35 Effect of Beta Weights Adjustments to the beta weights have the effect of raising or lowering the height of the predictor while keeping the shape constant

36 Dynamic Example

37 The beta weight is NOT a correlation
correlations measure goodness of fit regardless of scale beta weights are a measure of scale small ß large r small ß small r large ß large r large ß small r

38 We create a GLM with 2 predictors
when 1=2 fMRI Signal + Residuals = + when 2=0.5 = Design Matrix x Betas “what we CAN explain” “how much of it we CAN explain” “what we CANNOT explain” = “our data” x + Statistical significance is basically a ratio of explained to unexplained variance

39 The “Linear” in GLM The GLM assumes that activation adds linearly
Much more on this next lecture Poldrack, Mumford & Nichols, 2011 fMRI Data Analysis

40 Correlated Predictors
Where possible, avoid predictors that are highly correlated with one another This is why we NEVER include a baseline predictor baseline predictor is almost completely correlated with the sum of existing predictors + r = -.53 = r = -.53 r = -.95 Two stimulus predictors Baseline predictor

41 Which model accounts for this data?
x β = 1 x β = 0 + OR + x β = 1 x β = 0 + + x β = 0 x β = -1 Because the predictors are highly correlated, the model is overdetermined and you can’t tell which beta combo is best

42 Orthogonalizing Regressors

43 Orthogonalizing Regressors
Outcome depends highly on which regressor goes into the model first Use only with caution! Poldrack, Mumford & Nichols, 2011 fMRI Data Analysis

44 = + Maximizing Your Power signal noise
As we saw earlier, the GLM is basically comparing the amount of signal to the amount of noise How can we improve our stats? increase signal decrease noise increase sample size (keep subject in longer)

45 How to Reduce Noise If you can’t get rid of an artifact, you can include it as a “predictor of no interest” to soak up variance Example: Some people include predictors from the outcome of motion correction algorithms Corollary: Never leave out predictors for conditions that will affect your data (e.g., error trials) This works best when the motion is uncorrelated with your paradigm (predictors of interest)

46 Including First Derivative
Some recommend including the first derivative of the HRF-convolved predictor can soak up some of the variance due to misestimations of the HRF

47 Now do you understand why we did temporal filtering?
raw data high- pass low- band- Poldrack, Mumford & Nichols, 2011 fMRI Data Analysis

48 Reducing Residuals

49 Alternative to Filtering
Rather than filtering low frequencies from our raw data, we can include a “discrete cosine basis set” that soaks up variance due to low frequency noise Poldrack, Mumford & Nichols, 2011 fMRI Data Analysis

50 Contrasts: Examples with Real Data

51 Sam’s Paradigm: Localizer for Ventral-Stream Visual Areas
Fusiform Face Area

52 Contrasts in the GLM We can examine whether a single predictor is significant (compared to the baseline) R L z = -20 We can also examine whether a single predictor is significantly greater than another predictor

53 Contrast Vectors Houses Faces Objects Bodies Scram Faces - Baseline +1
+1 Faces - Houses -1 Faces - Objects Faces - Bodies Faces - Scrambled

54 Balanced Contrasts Unbalanced Balanced β 1 2 1 1 1 Condition Contrast
-1 +1 Σ=-3 β 1 2 Σ=-2 Contrast -1 +4 Σ=-0 β 1 2 8 Σ=4 If you do not balance the contrast, you are comparing one condition vs. the sum of all the others If you balance the contrast, you are comparing one condition vs. the average of all the others

55 Problems with Bulk Contrasts
β β 1 2 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 .5 Condition Condition Balanced: Faces vs. Other Balanced: Faces vs. Other Contrast -1 +4 Σ=0 β 1 2 8 Σ=4 Contrast -1 +4 Σ=0 β 2 0.5 -2 8 -0.5 Σ=1.5 Bulk contrasts can be significant if only a subset of conditions differ

56 Conjunctions (sometimes called Masking)
Houses Faces Objects Bodies Scram Faces - Baseline +1 Faces - Houses -1 Faces - Objects Faces - Bodies Faces - Scrambled AND AND AND AND To describe this in text: [(Faces > Baseline) AND (Faces > Houses) AND (Faces > Objects) AND (Faces > Bodies) AND (Faces > Scrambled)]

57 Conjunction Example Faces – Houses Faces – Objects Faces – Bodies
Scrambled Faces – Baseline Superimposed Maps Conjunction

58 P Values for Conjunctions
If the contrasts are independent: e.g., [(Faces > Houses) AND (Scrambled > Baseline)] pcombined = (psinglecontrast)numberofcontrasts e.g., pcombined = (0.05)2 = If the contrasts are non-independent: e.g., [(Faces > Houses) AND (Faces > Baseline)] pcombined is less straightforward to compute

59 Real Voxel: GLM Here’s the time course from a voxel in right FFA (defined by conjunction) GLM Data, Model, and Residuals dfpredictors = # of predictors dfresidual = dftotal - dfpredictors dftotal = #volumes - 1 262 volumes (time points) GLM predictors account for (0.784)2 = 61% of variance

60 e.g., tFace = βFace/seFace
Real Voxel: Betas t = β/se e.g., tFace = βFace/seFace tFace = 1.371/0.076 = t(5,261)=  p <

61 Real Voxel: Contrasts Σ[Contrast x β] = 0 x 0.964 + 1 x 1.371
= – 0.687 = 0.684

62 Dealing with Faulty Assumptions

63 What’s this #*%&ing reviewer complaining about?!
Correction for multiple comparisons Correction for serial correlations only necessary for data from single subjects not necessary for group data

64 Types of Errors Type I Error HIT Type II Error Correct Rejection
Is the region truly active? Does our stat test indicate that the region is active? Yes No HIT Type I Error Type II Error Correct Rejection p value: probability of a Type I error e.g., p <.05 “There is less than a 5% probability that a voxel our stats have declared as “active” is in reality NOT active Slide modified from Duke course

65 Dead Salmon 130,000 voxels no correction for multiple comparisons
poster at Human Brain Mapping conference, 2009 130,000 voxels no correction for multiple comparisons

66 Fishy Headlines

67 Mega-Multiple Comparisons Problem
Typical 3T Data Set 30 slices x 64 x 64 = 122,880 voxels of (3 mm)3 If we choose p < 0.05… 122,880 voxels x 0.05 = approx voxels should be significant due to chance alone We can reduce this number by only examining voxels inside the brain ~64,000 voxels (of (3 mm)3) x 0.05 = 3200 voxels significant by chance

68 Possible Solutions to Multiple Comparisons Problem
Bonferroni Correction small volume correction Cluster Correction False Discovery Rate Gaussian Random Field Theory Test-Retest Reliability

69 Bonferroni Correction
divide desired p value by number of comparisons Example: desired p value: p < .05 number of voxels in brain: 64,000 required p value: p < .05 / 64,000  p < Variant: small-volume correction only search within a limited space brain cortical surface region of interest reduces the number of voxels and thus the severity of Bonferroni Drawback: overly conservative assumes that each voxel is independent of others not true – adjacent voxels are more likely to be sig in fMRI data than non-adjacent voxels

70 Cluster Correction falsely activated voxels should be randomly dispersed set minimum cluster size (k) to be large enough to make it unlikely that a cluster of that size would occur by chance some algorithms assume that data from adjacent voxels are uncorrelated (not true) some algorithms (e.g., Brain Voyager) estimate and factor in spatial smoothness of maps cluster threshold may differ for different contrasts Drawbacks: handicaps small regions (e.g., subcortical foci) more than large regions researcher can test many combinations of p values and k values and publish the one that looks the best

71 False Discovery Rate “controls the proportion of rejected hypotheses that are falsely rejected” (Type II errors) standard p value (e.g., p < .01) means that a certain proportion of all voxels will be significant by chance (1%) FDR uses q value (e.g., q < .01), meaning that a certain proportion of the “activated” (colored) voxels will be significant by chance (1%) Drawbacks very conservative when there is little activation; less conservative when there is a lot of activation

72 Gaussian Random Field Theory
Fundamental to SPM If data are very smooth, then the chance of noise points passing threshold is reduced Can correct for the number of “resolvable elements” (“resels”) rather than number of voxels Drawback: Requires smoothing Slide modified from Duke course

73 Test-Retest Reliability
Perform statistical tests on each half of the data The probability of a given voxel appearing in both purely by chance is the square of the p value used in each half e.g., .001 x .001 = Alternatively, use the first half to select an ROI and the second half to test your hypothesis Drawback: By splitting your data in half, you’re reducing your statistical power to see effects

74 Sanity Checks: “Poor Man’s Bonferroni”
For casual data exploration, not publication Jack up the threshold till you get rid of the schmutz (especially in air, ventricles, white matter – may be real) If you have a comparison where one condition is expected to produce much more activity than the other, turn on both tails of the comparison If two areas are symmetrically active, they’re less likely to be due to chance (only works for bilateral areas) Jody’s rule of thumb: “If ya can’t trust the negatives, can ya trust the positives?” Too subjective for serious use Example: MT localizer data Moving rings > stationary rings (orange) Stationary rings > moving rings (blue)

75 Have We Been So Obsessed with Limiting Type I Error that Type II Error is Out of Control?
Is the region truly active? Does our stat test indicate that the region is active? Yes No HIT Type I Error Type II Error Correct Rejection Slide modified from Duke course

76 Comparison of Methods simulated data uncorrected -high Type I
-low Type II Bonferroni -low Type I -high Type II FDR -low Type I -low Type II Poldrack, Mumford & Nichols, 2011 fMRI Data Analysis

77 Strategies for Exploration vs. Publication
Deductive approach Have a specific hypothesis/contrast planned Run all your subjects Run the stats as planned Publish Inductive approach Run a few subjects to see if you’re on the right track Spend a lot of time exploring the pilot data for interesting patterns “Find the story” in the data You may even change the experiment, run additional subjects, or run a follow-up experiment to chase the story While you need to use rigorous corrections for publication, do not be overly conservative when exploring pilot data or you might miss interesting trends Random effects analyses can be quite conservative so you may want to do exploratory analyses with fixed effects (and then run more subjects if needed so you can publish random effects)

78 What’s this #*%&ing reviewer complaining about?!
Correction for multiple comparisons Correction for serial correlations only necessary for data from single subjects not necessary for group data stay tuned to find out why: Group Data lecture

79 Correction for Temporal Correlations
When analyzing a single subject, degrees of freedom = number of volumes – 1 e.g., if our run has 200 volumes (400 s long if TR = 2), then df = 199 Statistical methods assume that each of our time points is independent. In the case of fMRI, this assumption is false. Even in a “screen saver scan”, activation in a voxel at one time is correlated with it’s activation within ~6 sec This artificially inflates your statistical significance.

80 Autocorrelation function
time To calculate the magnitude of the problem, we can compute the autocorrelation function on the residuals For a voxel or ROI, correlate its time course with itself shifted in time Plot these correlations by the degree of shift original shift by 1 volume shift by 2 volumes If there’s no autocorrelation, function should drop from 1 to 0 abruptly – pink line The points circled in yellow suggest there is some autocorrelation, especially at a shift of 1, called AR(1)

81 BV can correct for the autocorrelation to yield revised (usually lower) p values
BEFORE AFTER

82 BV Preprocessing Options

83 Temporal Smoothing of Data
We have the option in our software to temporally smooth our data (i.e., remove high temporal frequencies or “low-pass filter”) However, I recommended that you not use this option Now do you understand why?

84 To Localize or Not to Localise?

85 To Localize or Not to Localise?
Neuroimagers can’t even agree how to SPELL localiser/localizer!

86 Methodological Fundamentalism
The latest review I received…

87 Approach #1: Voxelwise Statistics
Run a statistical contrast for every voxel in your search volume. Correct for multiple comparisons. Find a bunch of blobs.

88 Voxelwise Approach: Example
Malach et al., 1995, PNAS Question: Are there areas of the human brain that are more responsive to objects than scrambled objects You will recognize this as what we now call an LO localizer, but Malach was the first to identify LO LO (red) responds more to objects, abstract sculptures and faces than to textures, unlike visual cortex (blue) which responds well to all stimuli LO activation is shown in red, behind MT+ activation in green

89 Approach #2: Region of interest (ROI) analysis
Identify a region of interest Functional ROI Anatomical Functional-Anatomical images from O’Reilly et al., 2012, SCAN Perform statistical contrasts for the ROI data in an INDEPENDENT data set Because the runs that are used to generate the area are independent from those used to test the hypothesis, liberal statistical thresholds (e.g., p < .05) can be used

90 Localizer Scan A separate scan conducted to identify functional regions of interest

91 Example of ROI Approach
Culham et al., 2003, Experimental Brain Research Does the Lateral Occipital Complex compute object shape for grasping? Step 1: Localize LOC Intact Objects Scrambled Objects

92 Example of ROI Approach
Culham et al., 2003, Experimental Brain Research Does the Lateral Occipital Complex compute object shape for grasping? Step 2: Extract LOC data from experimental runs Grasping Reaching NS p = .35 NS p = .31

93 Example of ROI Approach
Very Simple Stats % BOLD Signal Change Left Hem. LOC Subject Grasping Reaching 1 0.02 0.03 2 0.19 0.08 3 0.04 0.01 4 0.10 0.32 5 1.01 -0.27 6 0.16 0.09 7 0.12 Then simply do a paired t-test to see whether the peaks are significantly different between conditions Extract average peak from each subject for each condition NS p = .35 NS p = .31 Instead of using % BOLD Signal Change, you can use beta weights You can also do a planned contrast in Brain Voyager using a module called the ROI GLM

94 Example: The Danger of ROI Approaches
Example 1: LOC may be a heterogeneous area with subdivisions; ROI analyses gloss over this Example 2: Some experiments miss important areas (e.g., Kanwisher et al., 1997 identified one important face processing area -- the fusiform face area, FFA -- but did not report a second area that is a very important part of the face processing network -- the occipital face area, OFA -- because it was less robust and consistent than the FFA.

95 Pros and Cons: Voxelwise Approach
Benefits Require no prior hypotheses about areas involved Include entire brain May identify subregions of known areas that are implicated in a function Doesn’t require independent data set Drawbacks Requires conservative corrections for multiple comparisons vulnerable to Type II errors Neglects individual differences in brain regions poor for some types of studies (e.g., topographic areas) Can lose spatial resolution with intersubject averaging Requires speculation about areas involved

96 Pros and Cons: ROI Approach
Benefits Extraction of ROI data can be subjected to simple stats Elimination of mega multiple comparisons problem greatly improves statistical power (e.g., p < .05) Hypothesis-driven Useful when hypotheses are motivated by other techniques (e.g., electrophysiology) in specific brain regions ROI is not smeared due to intersubject averaging Important for discriminating abutting areas (e.g., V1/V2) Easy to analyze and interpret Can be useful for dissecting factorial design data in an unbiased manner Drawbacks Neglects other areas that may play a fundamental role If multiple ROIs need to be considered, you can spend a lot of scan time collecting localizer data (thus limiting the time available for experimental runs) Works best for reliable and robust areas with unambiguous definitions Sometimes you can’t find an ROI in some subjects Selection of ROIs can be highly subjective and error-prone

97 A Proposed Resolution There is no reason not to do BOTH ROI analyses and voxelwise analyses ROI analyses for well-defined key regions Voxelwise analyses to see if other regions are also involved Ideally, the conclusions will not differ If the conclusions do differ, there may be sensible reasons Effect in ROI but not voxelwise perhaps region is highly variable in stereotaxic location between subjects perhaps voxelwise approach is not powerful enough Effect in voxelwise but not ROI perhaps ROI is not homogenous or is context-specific

98 The War of Non-Independence

99 Finding the Obvious A priori probability of getting JQKA sequence = (1/13)4 = 1/28,561 A posteriori probability of getting JQKA sequence = 1/1 = 100% Non-independence error occurs when statistical tests performed are not independent from the means used to select the brain region Arguments from Vul & Kanwisher, book chapter in press

100 Non-independence Error
Egregious example Identify Area X with contrast of A > B Do post hoc stats showing that A is statistically higher than B Act surprised!!! More subtle example of selection bias Do post hoc stats showing that A is statistically higher than C and C is statistically greater than B Arguments from Vul & Kanwisher, book chapter in press Figure from Kriegeskorte et al., 2009, Nature Neuroscience

101 Double Dipping & How to Avoid It
Kriegeskorte et al., 2009, Nature Neuroscience surveyed 134 papers in prestiguous journals 42% showed at least one example of non-independence error

102 Correlations Between Individual Subjects’ Brain Activity and Behavioral Measures
Sample of Critiqued Papers: Eisenberg, Lieberman & Williams, 2003, Science measured fMRI activity during social rejection correlated self-reported distress with brain activity found r = .88 in anterior cingulate cortex, an area implicated in physical pain perception concluded “rejection hurts” social exclusion > inclusion

103 “Voodoo Correlations”
The original title of the paper was not well-received by reviewers so it was changed even though some people still use the term Voodoo 2009 reliability of personality and emotion measures: r ~ .7 reliability of activation in a given voxel: r ~ .7 highest expected behavior: fMRI correlation is ~.74 so how can we have behavior: fMRI correlations of r ~.9?!

104 “Voodoo Correlations”
"Notably, 53% of the surveyed studies selected voxels based on a correlation with the behavioral individual-differences measure and then used those same data to compute a correlation within that subset of voxels." Vul et al., 2009, Perspectives on Psychological Science

105 Avoiding “Voodoo” Use independent means to select region and then evaluate correlation Do split-half reliability test WARNING: This is reassuring that the result can be replicated in your sample but does not demonstrate that result generalizes to the population

106 Is the “voodoo” problem all that bad?
High correlations can occur in legitimately analyzed data Did voxelwise analyses use appropriate correction for multiple comparisons? then result is statistically significant regardless of specific correlation Is additional data being used for inference purposes? if they pretend to provide independent support, that’s bad presentation purposes? alternative formats can be useful in demonstrating that data is clean (e.g., time courses look sensible; correlations are not driven by outliers)


Download ppt "fMRI Analysis with emphasis on the General Linear Model"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google