Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Jason A. Cromer, Jefferson E. Roy, Earl K. Miller  Neuron 

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Jason A. Cromer, Jefferson E. Roy, Earl K. Miller  Neuron "— Presentation transcript:

1 Representation of Multiple, Independent Categories in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex 
Jason A. Cromer, Jefferson E. Roy, Earl K. Miller  Neuron  Volume 66, Issue 5, Pages (June 2010) DOI: /j.neuron Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

2 Figure 1 Stimulus Set and Behavioral Task
(A) Morphing allowed parameterization of sample images. An example morph line between Cat prototype c1 and Dog prototype d2 displays images at the morph steps used for recording. Intermediate images were a mix of the two prototypes. Those images comprised of greater than 50% of one category (marked by the “Category Boundary”) where to be classified as a member of that category. (B) Stimuli came from two independent category sets, Animals and Cars. The Animal category set was divided into “Cats” versus “Dogs” and the Car category set had “Sports Cars” and “Sedans” categories. Both sets were comprised of four prototype images (two from each category as shown) as well as images along four between category morph lines. (C) The delayed match to category task required monkeys to respond to whether a test stimulus matched the category of the sample stimulus. During the sample and delay periods, the monkeys must hold in memory the category of the sample stimulus but the outcome of the trial is unknown. Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

3 Figure 2 Behavior and Single Neuron Example
(A and B) Performance of both monkeys on the delayed match to category task with multiple, independent category distinctions across all recording sessions. Monkeys were able to categorize both Animals and Cars exceptionally well and displayed a hallmark step function in behavior at the category boundary. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. (C) A single PFC neuron showed distinct firing for stimuli of one category (e.g., Sedans) versus the other category (e.g., Sports Cars). Note how all morph percentages on either side of the category boundary (50%) grouped together (e.g., blue versus red lines), despite the fact that sample images near the boundary line (60%/40%, dark lines) were closer in physical similarity. Thus, this neuron responded to the category membership of the stimuli rather than their visual properties. This individual PFC neuron multitasked, categorizing both Animals (Cats versus Dogs) and Cars (Sedans versus Sports Cars) during the late delay interval. Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

4 Figure 3 Category Selectivity by Morph Level
Normalized neuronal firing of category sensitive neurons sorted by the percentage of each neuron's preferred category that made up the sample image. During fixation (baseline) no category effect is present, but during the sample, delay, and test periods there is a significant difference in firing across the category boundary (but not within). The same hallmark step function as seen in the monkeys' behavior is seen in the neural population activity. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate that bars were significantly different (t tests, p < 0.05) from each bar on the opposite side of the category boundary. Bars on the same side of category boundary were never significantly different. Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

5 Figure 4 Category Selectivity across All Images
Correlation value (r) were computed for all neurons' mean responses to all possible pairings of sample images from the same category set (images 1–20, either Animals or Cars). Figure key: correlation values were then used to define the color of each square in a matrix representing these image pairings. The matrix was arranged such that images 1–10 came from one category (e.g., “Sports Cars”) and images 11–20 came from the opposite category (e.g., “Sedans”). The matrix was further subdivided such that every five images came from the same prototype. (A) The average activity of PFC Animal sensitive neurons to images from the same category was highly correlated (similar)—as seen in the warm-colored squares, whereas correlations to images from different categories was negative—deep blue squares. This was true for both the Animal category distinction (left panel) as well as for the Car category distinction (right panel), despite the fact that Car sensitivity was not a factor in selecting the neurons. Thus, Animal sensitive neurons multitask and also convey information about the Car category. (B) Car sensitive neurons display strong selectivity to the Car category distinction as well as sensitivity to the Animal category distinction. Again, activity is strongest for the expected distinction (Cars), but clearly evident for the nonselected distinction (Animals) due to multitasking neurons. (C) Noncategory sensitive neurons had low correlation between the sample images and these correlations were nonsignificant across categories (between images 1 and 10 and images 11 and 20). p values indicate significance between categories as determined by permutation tests (see text). See also Figures S1 and S2. Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

6 Figure 5 ROCs for the Recorded PFC Population
(A) ROC values for each of the recorded 455 PFC neurons are shown, with the same neuron depicted in the same row on the left and right panels. Bright orange colors indicate high category sensitivity. Neurons identified previously as sensitive to the Cats versus Dogs category distinction (via t test) are sorted on max ROC (left panel, below the dashed white line). The same neurons have the highest ROC values for the Sports Cars versus Sedans category distinction (right panel). (B) Data as in (A) now realigned on the max ROC for Car sensitive neurons. Again, the majority of selectivity for both category distinctions is in the lower portion of the panels below the dashed white line, even though the Animal ROCs are aligned to match the Car sensitivity. Thus, the majority of PFC category sensitive neurons multitask and encode both category distinctions. See also Figure S3. Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

7 Figure 6 Mean ROCs for Each Category Scheme
Mean ROC values for all recorded neurons to both category distinctions. Data points are color coded based on significant category sensitivity via t test. Values closer to the origin indicate weaker category sensitivity for the given distinction. Data points were equally distributed in all four quadrants, indicating equal neuronal preference for all categories. Data points furthest from the origin in all four directions were from those multitasking neurons sensitive to both the Animal and Car category schemes (yellow circles). Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

8 Figure 7 Multitasking of Neurons for Independent and Competing Category Schemes (A) In the current study, when monkeys categorized two independent category sets (Animals and Cars), 44% of category sensitive neurons showed category effects for both category sets. (B) This is in contrast to another study from our laboratory (Roy et al., 2010) in which the same images were categorized under two different (orthogonal) category sets: Animals (“Cats versus Dogs”) or Animals 2 (a new category distinction based on two unique combinations of a cat and dog prototype). The Cat and Dog categories were the same used in this study. In the Roy et al. (2010) experiment with orthogonal category sets, fewer PFC neurons (24%) showed category sensitivity for both category distinctions. Neuron  , DOI: ( /j.neuron ) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions


Download ppt "Jason A. Cromer, Jefferson E. Roy, Earl K. Miller  Neuron "

Similar presentations


Ads by Google